Home |
Search |
Today's Posts |
#1
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Since we all know you are in desparate need of more information on how and
why DBT's are used by Sean Olive and Floyd Toole I include the following for your edification. Audio - Science in the Service of Art by Floyd E. Toole, Ph.D. Vice President Engineering, Harman International Senior Vice President Acoustical Engineering, JBL and Infinity INTRODUCTION Audio products must sound good. That is a given. However, the determination of what constitutes "good sound" is a matter that has been controversial. Some assert that it is a matter of personal taste, that our opinions of sound quality are as variable as our tastes in "wine, persons or song". This would place audio manufacturers in the category of artists, trying to appeal to a varying public "taste". Others, like the author, take a more pragmatic view, namely that artistry is the domain of the instrument makers and musicians and that it is the role of audio devices to capture, store and reproduce their art with as much accuracy as technology allows. The audio industry then becomes the messenger of the art. Interestingly, though, this process has created new "artists", the recording engineers, who are free to editorialize on the impressions of direction, space, timbre and dynamics of the original performance, as perceived by listeners through their audio systems. Other creative opportunities exist at the point of reproduction, as audiophiles tailor the fundamental form of the sound field in listening rooms by selecting loudspeakers of differing timbral signatures and directivities, and by adjusting the acoustics of the listening space with furnishings or special acoustical devices. To design audio products, engineers need technical measurements. Historically, measurements have been viewed with varying degrees of trust. However, in recent years, the value of measurements has increased dramatically, as we have found better ways to collect data, and as we have learned how to interpret the data in ways that relate more directly to what we hear. Measurements inevitably involve objectives, telling us when we are successful. Some of these design objectives are very clear, and others still need better definition. All of them need to be moderated by what is audible. Imperfections in performance need not be unmeasurably small, but they should be inaudible. Achieving this requires knowledge of psychoacoustics, the relationship between what we measure and what we hear. This is a work in progress, but considerable gains have been made. This paper reviews scientific work performed by the author and his colleagues, that aimed to determine the extent to which listeners agreed on their preferences in sound quality and, beyond that, to identify relationships between listener preferences and measurable performance parameters of loudspeakers. Given that loudspeakers, listeners and rooms form a complex acoustical system, some of the effort was necessarily devoted to identifying the aspects of performance that maximize the performance of the entire system in real world circumstances. ART, SCIENCE AND AUDIO Science has allowed us to do some remarkable things. It has enabled us to travel safely back and forth to the moon, to drive around Mars, to harness atomic energy for peace and to unleash it for war. Its principles allow engineers to build tall buildings, immense bridges, and vehicles that roll on the earth and those that fly through air and space. These and most other aspects of scientific accomplishment are associated with "things", or theoretical concepts far removed from our everyday lives. However, that same foundation of scientific knowledge, principles, and methodology has given us the ability to enjoy the excitement, emotions and sheer beauty of music, whenever or wherever the mood strikes us. Music, itself, is art, pure and simple. The composers, performers and the creators of the musical instruments are artists and craftsmen. Through their skills, we are the grateful recipients of sounds that can create and change moods, that can excite and animate us to dance and sing, and that form an important component of our memories. Music is part of all of us and of our lives. However, in spite of its many capabilities, science cannot describe music. Beyond the crude notes on a sheet of music, science has no dimensions to measure the evocative elements of a good tune. It cannot technically describe why Pavarotti's tenor voice is so revered, or why the sound of a Stradivarius violin is held as an example of how it should be done. Nor can science distinguish, by measurement, the mellifluous qualities of trumpet intonations by Wynton Marsalis, and those of a music student who simply hits the notes. Those are distinctions that must be made subjectively, by listening. Some scientific effort has gone into musical instruments and, as a result, we are getting better at imitating the desirable aspects of superb instruments in less expensive ones. We are also getting better at electronically synthesizing the sounds of acoustical instruments. However, the determination of what is aesthetically pleasing, remains firmly based in subjectivity and the arts. Our audio industry is based on a seemingly simple sequence of events. We capture a musical performance with microphones, whose outputs are blended into an electronic message stored on tape or disc, which is subsequently reproduced through two or more loudspeakers. This simple description disguises a process that is enormously complicated. We know from experience that, in some ways, the process is remarkably good. For decades we have enjoyed reproduced music of all kinds with fidelity sufficient to, at times, bring tears to the eyes, and send chills down the spine. Still, critics of audio systems can sometimes point to timbral characteristics that are not natural, that change the sound of voices and instruments. They point to noises and distortions that were not in the original sounds, rendering even the most eloquent tunes in a brittle and harsh fashion. They note that closing the eyes does not result in a perception that the listener is involved in the performance, enveloped in the acoustical ambiance of a concert hall or jazz club. They point out that stereo, as we have known it, is an antisocial system - only a single listener can hear the reproduction as it was created. For all of these criticisms there are solutions, some here and now, and some under development. All of the solutions are based on science. How can science, a cold and calculating endeavor if ever there were one, help with delivering the emotions of great music? Because, in the space between the performers and the audience, music exists as sound waves. Sound waves are physical entities, subject to physical laws, amenable to technical measurement and description and, in most important ways, predictable. The physical science of acoustics allows us to understand the behavior of sound waves as they travel from the musician to the listener, whether the performance is "live" or recorded. To capture those sound waves, with all of the musical nuances intact, we need transducers to convert the variations in sound pressure (the sound waves) that impinge on them into exact electrical analogs. Microphone design uses the science of electroacoustics a blend of mechanical and electrical engineering with a dose of physics. Once the signal is in the electrical domain, the science and engineering expertise in electronics is brought to bear in preserving the integrity of the musical signal. Contamination by noise and distortions of any kind must be avoided, as it is prepared for storage and when it is recalled from storage during playback. Emerging from the tape or disc recorder, radio or TV, the musical signal is too feeble to be of any practical use, so we amplify it (more electronics), giving it the power to drive loudspeakers or headphones (more electroacoustics) that convert the electrical signal back into sound. Again, this all must be done without adding to or subtracting from the signal, or else it will not sound the way it should. Finally, the sound waves radiating from the loudspeakers propagate through the listening room to our ears. If there is a seriously weak link in this entire process, this is probably it. Rooms in our homes are different from any that would have been used for live performances, or for monitoring the making of a recording. Our eyes and our ears both recognize that. The acoustical properties of rooms, large and small, are in the scientific domain of architectural acoustics, and from that we could predict that there would be problems of the kind that we experience. The complex interactions between room boundaries and speaker directivity at middle and high frequencies, and speaker and listener position at low frequencies are powerful influences in what we hear. With careful acoustical design and electronic signal manipulations, we are finding ways to make speakers more "friendly" to rooms, both in recording studios and in homes. THE STEREO PRESENT AND THE MULTICHANNEL FUTURE In stereo there are only two loudspeakers to reconstruct an illusion of the complex three-dimensional sound field that existed in the original hall, club or studio. The choice of two channels was based on the limitations, then, of what could be stored in the groove of an LP. Even at that time, it was known that two channels were insufficient to recreate truly convincing illusions of a three-dimensional acoustical performance. The old argument that we only have two ears is valid only in the context of binaural recordings, where it is necessary to send specific sounds to each ear independently. The excitement and realism of the reproduction is enhanced if we have more channels and more loudspeakers. This is a relatively new development in music recording and, understandably, it will take some time for artists and recording engineers to learn how to use the new format. Examples from the abortive quadraphonics era, and from some current efforts, show that not everyone has learned how to merge multichannel technology with good taste. However, properly used, the combination of digital discrete multichannel technology, and the appropriate loudspeakers can provide the basis for some amazing sound experiences - both realistic and contrived. And, what might the appropriate loudspeakers be? Two-channel stereo, as we have known it, is not a "system" of recording and reproduction. The only "rule" is that there are two channels. At the recording end of the chain, there are many quite different theories and practices of miking and mixing the live performance - ranging from the purist simplicity of two coincident microphones to multi-microphone, multi-track, pan-potted and electronically-reverberated mono. They all can be great fun, some of them even very good, but they are all different. At the reproduction end of things, loudspeakers have taken many forms: forward facing, bipolar (bidirectional in phase), dipolar (bidirectional out-of-phase), omnidirectional, and a variety of multi-directional variants. These, and various sum/difference and delay devices, have been employed in attempts to coax, from a spatially-deprived medium, a rewarding sense of space and envelopment. In the two-channel world, therefore, the artists could not anticipate how their performances would sound in homes. It was left to the end user to create something pleasant. Stereo, therefore, is not an encode/decode system, but a basis for individual experimentation. Nowadays we have elegant active-matrix technologies, such as Lexicon's Logic 7, and Citation's 6-Axis that allow us to play conventional stereo recordings through multichannel systems, to introduce us to being truly enveloped in sound. Whether the experience is realistic, or even tasteful, will depend on how the recording was made - remember, there are no standards in stereo. All of this will get much better when recordings are created to be reproduced through multiple channels. Then, if we do not like what we hear, we can legitimately complain to the artist. Among the discoveries in this new era, may be that some of the loudspeaker designs that were flattering to stereo recordings, will be less appropriate for multichannel sound. For example, many listeners have come to favor loudspeakers having wide dispersion, or even multi-directional radiating characteristics, for stereo recordings. The principle at work here is that the reflected sounds in the listening room embellish the sounds from the two loudspeakers, pleasantly enhancing the impressions of acoustical warmth, depth and spaciousness. This is as it should be. Multichannel recordings are created with the knowledge that there are loudspeakers positioned around the room in locations optimized to create different directional and spatial illusions. This is a powerful advantage, in that it permits the artist to create an enormous range of effects: first, in movies: .. close-up speech, intimate whispers (listener in a strong direct sound field), .. being surrounded by reverberant space or cheering fans at a game or concert (multidirectional discrete sounds), .. fly-overs, drive-bys, ricochets, etc. (multidirectional discrete sounds), and in musical performances: .. being in a superb concert hall or club (direct sounds from an orchestra in front, with reflected and uncorrelated sounds from beside and behind), and .. being in the middle of a band (multidirectional direct sounds). These will be convincing only if the loudspeakers can deliver the appropriate sounds to the listeners' ears. If this "spatial dynamic range", as I call it, is to be achieved in normal - i.e. not acoustically treated - listening rooms, we will very likely need loudspeakers of differing directivities in different locations and, if we are truly fussy, we will need more than five channels. We have begun a voyage of discovery and, depending on how we approach it, it can be long and tortuous, or short and sweet. If it is approached with an appropriate blend of art and science, it could be the latter. It would be wonderful if the music industry could agree on a standard methodology for multichannel sound. However, based on experience, that is extremely unlikely. At this point it is necessary to introduce the science of psychoacoustics, the study of relationships between physical sounds and the perceptions that result from them. Psychoacoustics allows us to understand and interpret measurements in ways that relate to what we hear. Such knowledge is absolutely necessary if we are to make significant progress in designing better products, especially when price is a concern. THE SCIENCE OF AUDIO Individual points of view are a part of human nature. They enrich our lives in many ways. It would be a boring world if we all were attracted to the same music, food, wine and people. However, it is a serious disadvantage if one is trying to design products that will be attractive to the majority of the consumer population. A point of view, commonly expressed, is that sound is "subjective", that we all "hear differently", and therefore not all of us prefer the same loudspeakers. It is also alleged that different nationalities, and regions have different preferences in sound. I have always regarded these assertions with suspicion because, if they were true, it would mean that there would be different pianos for each of these regions, different trumpets, bassoons and kettle drums. Vocalists would change how they sang when they were in Germany, Britain, and the U.S. I wonder what Pavarotti's Japanese timbre sounds like? I don't believe it . . .. and it doesn't happen that way. The entire world enjoys the same musical instruments and voices in live performance, and the recording industry sends the same recordings throughout the world. True, from time to time, there have been regional influences that have made differences. I can recall the "east coast / west coast" sounds associated with some powerful brands in those locations in the USA. In Britain, the British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) provided loudspeaker designs that became the paradigm for a few years. Everywhere, there are magazines and reviewers that are influential. All of these factors change with time. In truth, they are really minor variations on a common theme. Underlying it all is a powerful attraction to reproduce sounds as accurately as possible. Now, what about those individual preferences in sound quality? This interesting issue was settled by conducting many, many listening tests, using many, many listeners and many, many loudspeakers. To reduce the influences of price, size and style - factors that we absolutely know can reveal individuality - all of the tests were conducted blind. Other physical and psychological factors known to be sources of bias were also well controlled [refs. 1-3]. The results were surprisingly clear. When the data were compiled, it turned out that most people, most of the time, liked and disliked the same loudspeakers. There were also differences in the way listeners performed; most were remarkably consistent in repeated evaluations of the same product, while some others changed their opinions of the same product at different listening sessions. 10 ONE STANDARD DEVIATION FOR 9 HIGH FIDELITY AND EXPERIENCED LISTENERS WITH F 8 STUDIO MONITOR NORMAL HEARING (3% - 5%) SPEAKERS I 7 FIGURE 1: Judged on a FIDELITY scale of 0 to 10, terrible to perfect, this figure shows results of many listeners evaluating many loudspeakers from different categories. To the same scale are shown, symbolically, the variability of repeated judgements by experienced listeners with hearing levels close to audiometric zero, and those who exhibited broadband hearing loss. Also shown is the variability of data combined from several listeners with normal hearing. Figure 1 shows, as might be expected, a rather large range in performance of the loudspeakers themselves. It also shows that, in repeated judgements, listeners with normal hearing exhibited standard deviations small enough for high statistical significance to be associated with small (about 0.5 scale unit) rating differences between products. This important observation is VARIABILITY JUDGMENT 0 10 20 30 BROADBAND HEARING LOSS (dB) paralleled by another, perhaps even more important one: that groups of such listeners closely agreed with each other. The finding that hearing loss is a factor is not surprising - listeners who cannot hear all of the sound must make less reliable judges. What is surprising is that the deterioration in performance is so rapid. It is well defined within hearing losses that would not be regarded as alarming by conventional audiometric criteria, see Figure 2. This difference, one assumes, is related to the difficulty of the task: judging sound accuracy, as opposed to understanding the spoken word. The hearing loss, in this case, was defined by the average threshold elevation at frequencies below 1kHz. Those exhibiting this form of hearing loss, also tended to have loss at high frequencies. High-frequency loss, by itself, was not a clearly correlated factor. Reference 4 covers this in detail. FIGURE 2 CHOOSE YOUR LISTENERS WITH CARE FIDELITY 10 LISTENERS WITH LOW 9 JUDGMENT VARIABILITY / NORMAL HEARING 8 7 6 LISTENERS WITH HIGH 5 JUDGMENT VARIABILITY / 4 HEARING LOSS 3 2 1 0 FIGURE 3: A comparison of loudspeaker FIDELITY evaluations performed by two groups, one with low variability in their judgments and the other with high judgment Listeners with hearing loss not only exhibit high judgment variability, they can also exhibit strong individualistic biases in their judgments. This comes as no surprise, since such individuals are really in search of a "prosthetic" loudspeaker that somehow compensates for their disability. Since the disabilities vary enormously, so do the biases. The evidence of Figure 3 is that the group of normal-hearing listeners substantially agree in their ratings. Interestingly, the second group shares the opinion of the truly good speakers, A and B. However, speakers C and D exhibit characteristics that are viewed as problems by the normal group, but about which the second group has substantially no opinion. Based on individual opinions, either C or D could be the best or worst speaker in the world. It appears that their disabilities prevented some of the listeners from hearing certain of the deficiencies. Sadly, some listeners who fall into the problem category are talented and knowledgeable musicians or audio professionals whose vocations may have contributed to their condition. However articulately their opinions are enunciated, their views are of value only to them, personally. The conclusion is clear. If there is any desire to extrapolate the results of a listening evaluation to the population at large, it is essential to use representative listeners. In this context, it appears to be adequate to employ listeners with broadband hearing levels within about 20 dB of audiometric zero. According to some large surveys, this is representative of about 75%, or more, of the population - an acceptable target audience for most commercial purposes. This is not an "elitist" criterion. PRACTICE MAKES PERFECT - USE TRAINED LISTENERS Listeners in the early tests gained much of their experience "on the job", while performing the tests. Some listeners were musicians, and others had professional audio experience, but most were simply audio enthusiasts. Probably the single most apparent deficiency of novice listeners was the lack of a vocabulary to describe what they heard. Without such descriptions, most listeners found it difficult to be analytical in forming their judgments, and to remember how various test products sounded. It was also clear that, without the prompting of a well-designed questionnaire, not all listeners paid attention to all perceptual dimensions, resulting in judgments that were highly selective. As the relationships between technically-measurable parameters and their audible importance became clearer, it was possible to design training sessions that focused on improving the ability of listeners to hear and to identify specific classes of problems in loudspeakers. With the aid of computers, this training has been refined to a self-administered procedure, which keeps track of the student's progress [5]. From this we have also been able to identify program material that is most revealing of the defects that are at issue, thus improving the efficiency and effectiveness of the tests. BLIND vs. SIGHTED TESTS - SEEING IS BELIEVING Knowledge of the products that are being evaluated is generally understood to be a powerful source of psychological bias. In scientific tests of many kinds, and even in wine tasting, considerable effort is expended to ensure the anonymity of the devices or substances being subjectively evaluated. In audio, though, things are more relaxed, and otherwise serious people persist in the belief that they can ignore such factors as price, size, brand, etc. In some of the "great debate" issues, like amplifiers, wires, and the like, there are assertions that disguising the product identity prevents listeners from hearing differences that are in the range of extremely small to inaudible. That debate shows no signs of slowing down. In the category of loudspeakers and rooms, however, there is no doubt that differences exist and are clearly audible. To satisfy ourselves that the additional rigor was necessary, we tested the ability of some of our trusted listeners to maintain objectivity in the face of visible information about the products. The results are very clear, and strongly supportive of the scientific view. Figure 4 shows that, in subjective ratings of four loudspeakers, the FIDELITY differences in ratings caused by knowledge of the products is as large or larger than those attributable to the differences in sound alone. The two left-hand striped bars are scores for loudspeakers that were large, expensive and impressive looking, the third bar is the score for a well designed, small, inexpensive, plastic three-piece system. The right-hand bar represents a moderately expensive product from a competitor that had been highly rated by respected reviewers. When listeners entered the room for the sighted tests, their positive verbal reactions to the big beautiful speakers and the jeers for the tiny sub/sat system BLIND SIGHTED FIGURE 4: A comparison of blind vs. sighted foreshadowed dramatic ratings shifts - in opposite evaluations of the same products by the same directions. The handsome competitor's system got a group of listeners. higher rating; so much for employee loyalty. Other variables were also tested, and the results indicated that, in the sighted tests, listeners substantially ignored large differences in sound quality attributable to position in the listening room and to program material. In other words, knowledge of the product identity was at least as important a factor in the tests as the principal acoustical factors. Incidentally, many of these listeners were very experienced and, some of them thought, able to ignore the visually-stimulated biases [6]. At this point, it is correct to say that, with adequate experimental controls, we are no longer conducting "listening tests", we are performing "subjective measurements". "ZOOMING IN" ON THE PROBLEMS Inevitably, as we make progress in improving products, the listening tests no longer include examples of bad performance. On the 0 to 10, junk to jewels, "fidelity" scale, therefore, one ends up listening exclusively to devices that score in the top ten percent, or so. When reminders of bad sound are removed from the tests, an interesting thing happens: listeners spontaneously expand the scaling of their responses to fill more of the range. In the absence of reminders about how bad things can really be, we become more critical of the relatively good sounds we are evaluating. 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 F I D E L I T 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 FIGURE 5: What happens when listeners evaluate products that are closely ranked at the top of a range of products. Without any "anchor products" to remind them of how things sound at lower ratings, the response scale expands to fill a "comfortable" range. A consequence of the phenomenon illustrated in Figure 5, is that, when they are auditioned in isolation, listeners tend to exaggerate the importance of small differences. If one is focusing attention on the small differences, that is a good thing. If, however, one is attempting to arrive at an evaluation of a phenomenon in a manner that relates to its importance in a total system, it is a distorted judgment. Real-world examples of this occur when, for example, one does comparisons of devices that are fundamentally similar. Many electronic devices fall into this category, as do loudspeakers that differ only in minor details. The listener impressions may be that there is an undisputed winner, a 9 versus a 6, but the reality is that, if there were any other variables in the test, the ratings may differ only by fractions of a point, and may even be in a different order. Because of this, another response-scaling method is used when we are interested only in the relative performance of devices. It is a preference scale. 1 9876 5 432 1 0 RATING PREFERENCE REALLY LIKE SLIGHT PREFERENCE LIKE NEITHER LIKE NOR DISLIKE MODERATE PREFERENCE DISLIKE REALLY DISLIKE STRONG PREFERENCE FIGURE 6: The preference rating scale that is used when comparing sounds on a relative, rather than an absolute, basis. On the right are the suggestions for rating differences according to the strength of the preference. The function of this kind of response scaling is to establish how listeners respond to differences between sounds. Since the scale is not "anchored" by reference to sounds known to be very good and very bad, there is nothing to indicate where they stand in any absolute sense. TECHNICAL MEASUREMENTS SUBJECTIVE MEASUREMENTS With statistically reliable, repeatable, numerical subjective ratings of loudspeakers in hand, it is an unavoidable temptation to look for orderly relationships with technical data on the same products. Figure 7 shows that the axial response of a loudspeaker needs to be very smooth, flat and wide-band in order to achieve high subjective ratings. One can rightly conclude that this is associated with a perception of timbral neutrality - a lack of coloration. But, there is more to the story. Since loudspeakers function in rooms, it is necessary that the sounds radiated in other directions also be well behaved, so that the ears receive similarly neutral sounds after single and multiple reflections from floor, walls, etc. The extension to the requirement, therefore, is that the off axis frequency responses, including sound power, also be well-behaved [7,8]. The design target, therefore, is a smooth, flat, axial response, with a constant directivity as a function of frequency. Just as axial behavior, by itself, is an insufficient criterion, so is sound power. FIGURE 7: A simplistic view of the relationship between the spatially-averaged axial frequency response of loudspeakers and their subjective ratings. This is a necessary, but not sufficient, criterion of excellence. Since absolute perfection in transducers and enclosures is still a remote possibility, it is important to be able to identify, in measurements, the presence and the audibility of defects such as resonances. In this way designers can choose to build the best product possible or, by making appropriate compromises, the best product at a given price level. The first step in this process is to develop a measurement method that allows the eyes to identify the presence and magnitude of defects that are audible. The second step is to identify the measured level at which the defect is audible - the detection threshold below which the defect ceases to be a problem. F I D E L I T Y 015L 15UP 15 DN FIGURE 8: A simple form of spatial averaging, in which measurements on several axes can be viewed individually, or in combination. A frequency response curve contains evidence of both resonances and acoustical interference. It is important to be able to identify which detailed features in the curve are attributable to each phenomenon. Why? Because, in a room, resonances will be easy to hear, and interference will be perceptually attenuated. Spatial averaging is a simple way to identify resonances 15R since, in the data for each of the microphone positions, the resonances will be relatively unchanged, while evidence of acoustical interference will change as a function of microphone position. When the collection of frequency response curves is averaged, the visual evidence of resonances remains, while that for the interference is diminished. It is a very simple, but very effective analytical method. 0 10 FIGURE 9: A frequency response measurement before (top curve) and after (bottom curve) spatial averaging. Data processing of multiple measurements, as in Figure 9, allows us to see, in high resolution, the amplitude and "Q" of resonances in complex mechanical and acoustical systems. These data were gathered in an anechoic chamber, where it is possible to measure with high resolution over the entire frequency range. A practical problem is that many measurements these days are made with FFT or TDS systems that timewindow the data so that anechoic measurements can be made in normal rooms. A result of the time windowing is that the frequency response data have limited frequency resolution, most noticeable at the low frequency end of the spectrum. As a result, it is not True Level CANNOT MEASURE WHAT WE HEAR possible to see high-Q phenomena at low and middle frequencies. 10 FIGURE 10: Identical high-Q resonances B (Q=50) at equal intervals between 20 Hz 0 and 20 kHz, adjusted to the threshold of audibility, as measured by an FFT measurement system having a time 20 50 1 00 500 1K 5K 10K 20K window of 17 ms, corresponding to a FREQUENCY (Hz) frequency resolution of 60 Hz. Resolution limitations of the kind shown in Figure 10, and worse, are common among loudspeaker designers and reviewers, because of the scarcity and expense of anechoic chambers, and the practical difficulties in measuring outdoors, in nature's own anechoic space. It means, simply, that many commonly-used and published measurements simply cannot reveal visual evidence of certain kinds of audible problems falling within a critical portion of the frequency range - that of the human voice. Another common measurement is one in which the audible frequency range is divided into equal fixedpercentage bandwidths, such as 1/3 octaves, or in which a high-resolution measurement is heavily smoothed, or spectrally averaged, on a continuous basis. These spectral-averaging devices have extremely limited utility in the design and evaluation process. Spatial averaging adds information, whereas spectral averaging removes spectral details, making curves look smoother and prettier. The sound, though, remains unchanged. Is it any wonder that some people mistrust measurements? Serious loudspeaker manufacturers need to be able to see, in measurements, anything that might result in an audible defect. In terms of the measurement of frequency response, it means expensive anechoic chambers, facilities for gathering data over an entire sphere, and elaborate post-processing of the data. Doing this quickly and accurately is neither inexpensive nor simple. THE AUDIBILITY OF RESONANCES Why is it that resonances are so important? Because they are the fundamental building blocks of almost all of the sounds we are interested in hearing. High-Q resonances define the pitches of voices and instruments. Medium- and low-Q resonances define the timbres of sounds, allowing us to distinguish between different voices and instruments. It is subtle differences in the resonant structure of sounds that are responsible for the nuances and shading of tone in musical sounds. Our ears are very highly attuned to the detection and evaluation of resonances, and it is therefore no surprise that listeners zero in on them as unwanted "editorializing" when they appear in loudspeakers. In order to be effective, design engineers need to know when a resonance is present, and if it is audible. PINK NOISEQ=50 Q=10 Q=1 The techniques described above, help to identify the presence of resonances. Reference 9 describes, in detail, how the audibility of resonances is related to measurements. FIGURE 11: The amplitude response measured in an otherwise perfect system, after a single resonance has been added at the threshold of detection, when listening to the most revealing signal, pink noise. dB In that study, resonances of different Q, at different frequencies, were added to an otherwise excellent system, and the amplitudes at which they 100 200 500 1K 2K 5K 10K were just detected by listeners were determined by FREQUENCY (Hz)experimentation. Different kinds of program material yielded different thresholds, as did different listening environments. Resonances reveal themselves in both the frequency-domain (amplitude and phase vs. frequency) and the time domain (impulse response / transient response). As defined by the Fourier Transform, if there is misbehavior in one domain, there will be misbehavior in the other, so we have two ways to look for problems. It is a matter of fact that high-Q resonances exhibit prolonged ringing in the time domain, and that low-Q resonances exhibit little ringing. The irony of this finding is that, as represented in conventional steady-state frequency response measurements, Figure 11, the low-Q resonances were detectable at much lower amplitudes than resonances of higher Q. If this is so, it means that a treasured belief is in jeopardy. The popular belief is that prolonged ringing, by itself, is a reliable indicator of an audible problem. To test this, we performed the measurements in the time domain, with the following results. INPUT SIGNAL FIGURE 12: Pulse responses of an otherwise perfect system (top curve) to which resonances have been added at the thresholds of detection. Q = 50 Looking at Figure 12, it is important to note that, in terms of audible changes in timbre, these responses are all equal. To the eyes, though, the long tail of the Q=50 resonance is quite alarming, and the Q=1 response appears Q = 10 almost perfect. How can this be so? One important factor would appear to be related to the portion of the spectrum influenced by a resonance. High-Q resonances are very narrow-band phenomena. In order for one of these to be energized by music, a sound would need to be closely centered on the frequency, and remain there long enough to impart significant energy to the resonant system. The higher the Q, the longer is the "build-up" period. In music, pitches are constantly changing, and voices and instrumental sounds frequently have vibrato, a fluctuating pitch. Such sounds would more often drive a low or medium Q resonance to maximum output, than they would a high-Q resonance. Statistically, therefore, with music and speech, lower-Q resonances would be heard more frequently than those of higher Q. The apparent contradiction between perception and measurement, then, begins with the observation that, as conventionally measured, the frequency response is a "steady-state" measurement, showing the resonance outputs at their maximum amplitude. With music, high-Q resonances are rarely driven to their maximum outputs, and so are less audible than the measurement indicates. The problem is not that the measurements are wrong, or irrelevant, it is that they are non-linearly related to the perceptual mechanism in humans, and therefore must be interpreted. Time-domain measurements are similarly problematic, since they suggest audibility in the prolongation of ringing. Such phenomena are wonderfully visible in pulse responses, tone-burst responses and the highly ornamental "waterfall" diagrams that digital measurement systems permit. Truth is that, without careful interpretation, these are just as misleading as the frequency responses. The conclusion is that, in practical situations, if a resonance does not make itself apparent in an accurate, high-resolution spatially-averaged, frequency-response measurement, then it is probably not audible. If a resonance is visible in a frequency response measurement, its audibility must be assessed by comparison with data from Figure 11, or better, from the detailed analysis in reference 9. An interesting fact now emerges: that the conventional method of specifying the excellence of frequency response -± x dB - is almost useless unless the tolerance is very, very small. For equal audibility, high-Q phenomena could be ± 5 dB, while moderate-Q resonances could be ± 3 dB and low-Q and other broadband deviations could be ± 0.5 dB. Clearly, frequency response curves must be interpreted, there is no simple "catchall" kind of tolerance specification that is truly meaningful. Such is life. Having identified the presence of a problematic resonance, it is the task of the design engineer to diagnose the cause, and to prescribe a remedy. All the while, it is essential to keep a close eye on costs. In this task, other specialized tools are needed, since the origins can be both acoustical and mechanical, and they can be associated with either the drivers or the enclosure. It is a curious phenomenon that the perception of "boxiness" in sound, may have nothing to do with the box itself. It is not uncommon for the offending resonance to have another origin. In the analysis of resonances, a laser interferometer/vibrometer is a powerful ally, in that it can show the complete vibratory behavior of a surface, such as a loudspeaker diaphragm or an entire surface of a loudspeaker enclosure. Vibration is important only if sound is radiated. Surfaces do not move uniformly, as a piston, at all frequencies, so measurement at a single point can be misleading. In practice, portions of the surface move in opposite directions, simultaneously. Consequently, some vibratory modes radiate sound very effectively, others less so, and some not at all. It is important not to go chasing after modes that simply cannot be audible. The combination of finite element analysis, in the design of diaphragms and enclosures, and modal analysis, after the prototype is built, allow engineers to be intelligent about the way they use shapes, materials, braces, etc. in reducing the audibility of resonances. The traditional method of playing safe, has been to build enclosures from massive, dense and stiff materials. If cost, size and weight are not considerations this method works well. However, enclosures with excellent acoustical performance can be built from mundane materials, at much lower cost, with due regard to modal prediction and analysis. In lower cost wooden enclosures, and especially with plastic enclosures, this becomes normal engineering practice. LOUDSPEAKERS AND ROOMS As important as the enclosures behind loudspeaker diaphragms are, it is the ones in front that, as often as not, give us serious problems. The listening room is the final audio component, and it is the one over which the loudspeaker manufacturer has little or no control. The first important step is to design loudspeakers so that they have a reasonable chance of sounding good in a room. In a room, listeners hear the direct sound first, followed quickly by early reflections from floor, ceiling and sidewalls. Then arrive the multitudes of sounds from many reflecting surfaces after several reflections - the reverberation. Ideally, all of these should reinforce a similar timbral signature in the mind of a listener. This can only happen if the loudspeakers are designed to radiate similar sounds in all of the relevant directions. In technical terms, this reduces to a requirement for constant directivity as a function of frequency. The directivity itself can take different forms for different applications, but is should not change with frequency. This is a difficult requirement to fill, and it requires special measurement techniques to allow engineers to judge their success as products are developed. In brief, it requires that we know: .. the nature of sounds radiated in the direction of the listener (the on-axis/listening-window performance), .. the nature of sounds that will be reflected from adjacent room surfaces (vertical and lateral "early-reflection" performance), and .. the nature of sounds that will generate the diffuse reverberant sound field (the sound power). From these the directivity index can be calculated. It is the combination of these that matter; any one by itself is insufficient data. Loudspeakers that meet these requirements also tend to be the ones that win listening tests, and there can be no better reinforcement for a methodology than that. One of the factors in listening tests is imaging, and that raises an issue for which there is not a complete answer at the moment: what is the ideal directivity? LOUDSPEAKERS FOR STEREO As discussed earlier, 40-some years with two-channel stereo have yielded nothing in the way of a clear direction. Although the majority of loudspeakers sold are traditional forward-facing "cone and dome" designs, it is also true that the majority of listeners are not very critical about the imaging of their systems. Among those who are, the "high end audiophiles", those designs figure strongly in their preferences. However, so do designs of very different kinds, like, dipole, bipole and directional horns. The perceptual consequences of speakers this diverse are not subtle. Bipole designs have approximately omnidirectional sound radiation properties, and therefore produce energetic reflected sound fields in listening rooms. Conventional forward-facing systems, and horn-loaded systems will place the listener in a sound field in which the direct sound is more prominent. The more directional the system, the more dominant will be the direct sound. It is probably correct to say that the majority of listeners find stereo to be pleasantly embellished if the room reflections are energetic. The sound tends to be open and spacious, with a good sense of depth, but the specific images can be rather vague - in other words, not unlike real concerts. A positive effect of this vagueness is that the stereo listening region is enlarged. However, there is also a category of listeners who respond unfavorably to this kind of reproduction, and prefer to have a very specific, almost pinpoint, sense of image position. Interestingly, this category includes many recording engineers who, in their studios, require that they be able to hear, very precisely, the results of their manipulations. Consequently, recording studios are often acoustically rather dead, and the loudspeakers directional (often horn loaded), or placed very close (so-called near-field listening). However, these same people, at home, frequently revert to the more spacious version of stereo. So, go figure. + - LOW BASS MID FREQUENCIES HIGH TREBLE + + FIGURE 13: Forward-firing (left), bidirectional-in-phase - Bipole (center) and bidirectional out-of-phase - Dipole (right), are just some of the very different directional patterns that are used in loudspeakers for stereo systems. The differences in imaging precision, spaciousness, and soundstage depth are not subtle. Once selected, though, the characteristics are applied to all kinds of music, whether it is appropriate or not. LOUDSPEAKERS FOR MULTICHANNEL AUDIO With the introduction of multichannel audio, things are no less complicated. Multichannel sound should mean that things become more controlled, that listeners stand a better chance of hearing the senses of direction and space that the artists created. In film applications, this is actually attempted. The film industry, has had basic standards for playback systems and environments for many years. THX improved on this, and attempted to translate it into the home. For the Dolby Surround films of that era, it was moderately successful. However, things now are much more confused, with Dolby Digital and DTS sound tracks and music recordings, and the Audio DVD around the corner. One senses an impending free-for-all in which anything goes. If it were approached logically, however, it seems that there is a scheme that makes sense. The purpose of multiple channels with loudspeakers located around the listeners, is to allow for a large variety of predictable localization and spatial effects. Since one of these is a sense of intimacy, wherein the sound from the front loudspeakers does not energize the listening room reverberant field, there is a requirement for front loudspeakers that are predominantly forward firing, with directional control in both vertical and horizontal planes. The wellknown THX requirement has been for some directional control in the vertical plane only. Many of the implementations have been somewhat less than ideal; simple vertical arrays of drivers cause severe lobing at the off-axis angles at which floor and ceiling bounces occur. If we are to address this issue properly, we need to incorporate horizontal directional control as well - our ears are in the horizontal plane, and it is horizontal reflections that are primarily responsible for the impressions of spaciousness [10]. We also need to focus on how well the speakers behave at the off-axis angles of importance - the adjacent boundary reflections and sound power. Predictable directional control is possible with horns, waveguide-loaded tweeters or complex twodimensional arrays, all of which can deliver excellent sound, using today's technology. The alternative, unattractive in most practical situations, is to move quantities of sound absorbing material into the room and cover large areas with it. The lack of attraction has two components, visual and acoustical. Visually, areas of sound absorber run contrary to popular themes of interior décor. Acoustically, absorbers dissipate sound energy that one has paid good money to create, thus making the speakers work even harder. In a multichannel system, the impressions of space and envelopment are provided by loudspeakers positioned to the sides of the listeners. Here we enter hostile territory, with monopoles, dipoles, tripoles and quadrapoles attempting to be the perfect solutions. Truth is that, while all of these names have meaning in the physics of sound, the products bearing them are only crude approximations to these forms of acoustic behavior. What really is at issue here is the matter of whether the listener should be in a predominantly direct or predominantly reverberant sound field from the surround loudspeakers. There cannot be a single correct answer for the loudspeaker configuration until there is agreement at the production end of the process. From the perceptual point of view, impressions of ambiguous localization and spaciousness exist when sounds arriving at the two ears are uncorrelated, containing many reflections. If the decorrelation is in the recording, or is added electronically, spaciousness can be perceived in headphone listening. It can also be convincing through conventional forward-facing surround speakers. However, much (most?) existing recorded material is deficient in this respect, and additional decorrelation tends to be beneficial. Using multidirectional (or multiple) surround speakers is a good method of adding decorrelation through multiple reflected sounds. The actual performance, however, is dominated by the geometry and reflective properties of the room. At present, movies are still made assuming that audiences will experience a multiplicity of speakers down the sides of the cinema, even though the digital discrete surround channels are all wideband and full range. At home, this argues for multidirectional surround speakers, strong electronic decorrelation, or multiple directradiating monopoles - i.e. business as usual. However, the notion of five identical loudspeakers is gaining favor. Multichannel music, still very much in its infancy, also supports the idea of five identical loudspeakers. DTS, in its multichannel music demos, goes a step further, and has been promoting the idea of moving the surround speakers to the rear of the room, mirroring the left and right fronts, and delivering sounds of individual musicians to these locations. At this point the audience divides into two camps - those who like having musicians behind them, and those who do not . . . and the feelings can be very strong. Some see this as a cause to criticize multichannel audio. This is where we must invoke the old adage: if you don't like the message, don't shoot the messenger. Multichannel audio systems are just delivery systems [11]. SPEAKERS FOR MUSIC AND SPEAKERS FOR MOVIES Are there fundamental differences between loudspeakers designed for movies and music? This oftrepeated concern is really the wrong question. The real question is: are there fundamental differences between loudspeakers designed for stereo and those designed for multichannel systems? The answer is that there may be. Can multichannel delivery systems work equally well for music and films? They must or we will have consumers up in arms, and for very good reasons. A good loudspeaker is a good loudspeaker, and there is no practical reason for different standards of performance in loudspeakers intended for films and music. There are reasons for differing directivities and, depending on how the loudspeakers are used, listeners will experience different imaging and spatial effects depending on the manner in which loudspeakers radiate their sound into rooms. That has always been true for stereo, and it remains so for multichannel systems. Thus, the choice of loudspeaker directivity should not alter ones expectations of inherent sound quality. GOOD BASS IN ROOMS - A BASIC PROBLEM At low frequencies the room interactions take on a special flavor because of the way acoustical standing waves (resonances) dominate what we hear. The quantity and quality of bass sound is as much or more determined by the room and how it is set up, as it is the speakers themselves [12,13,14,15]. This is an enormous frustration for speaker manufacturers. Once the product is in a box, on a truck, we have lost control of how the speaker will sound at low frequencies. Gaining some control, has been the objective of several projects and products over the years. Short of hiring an acoustical consultant, and being willing to rearrange the furniture and possibly the walls, what can be done? The short answer is "equalize". At this point, some people's hackles are rising, I am sure. Equalization has acquired a bad reputation over the years. To some proponents, it is a "cure all"; set up a microphone, follow the dancing lights of a "real-time analyzer", reach for the trusty multifilter equalizer and create a pretty curve. Such an exercise is almost certainly doomed to disappoint. Steady-state room curves are "dumb" measurements, in that the microphone simply adds up all of the incoming sounds, from whatever direction, after whatever time. One-third octave analysis, which is typical of these devices, is a crude measurement. Two ears and a brain are much more sensitive and analytical, directionally, temporally and spectrally. To be successful, equalization should address the problems it has the potential to remedy. There are some things equalization can and cannot do: 1. It cannot make a poor speaker sound good. With laboratory measurements to work from it might make a good speaker sound better. However, there is nothing that an average consumer can measure in a listening room that would reveal problems in a speaker that can be repaired by equalization. If the speaker has been competently designed, it should probably be left alone at frequencies above about 300 to 500 Hz, whatever the room-curves look like. 2. At frequencies below about 300 to 500 Hz, the system performance is dictated by the shape and size of the room, and the position of the speaker and listener within it. Three factors are interactively operational he .. solid-angle gains (the proximity of the speaker and listener to adjacent room boundaries: walls and floor.) Addressing this requires a broadband "tone-control" kind of equalization. .. room resonances, which can cause strong peaks and dips, some with quite high "Q". Before these can be addressed, the resonances must be identified within a complicated confusion of peaks and dips caused by acoustical interference. It is always unwise to try to fill dips - acoustical cancellations can be "bottomless pits". Prominent peaks can be individually addressed with parametric filters set to the appropriate center frequency, Q, and gain/loss. Identifying those parameters accurately requires highresolution measurements; much more detail than is revealed by the traditional 1/3-octave "real-time analyzers". .. Acoustical interference caused by the interaction of many reflected sounds within the room. These are non-minimum-phase phenomena, and they cannot be addressed with equalization. Fortunately, our ears are less sensitive to their effects than our measurement systems, so we really need to find a measurement process that diminishes their visibility. Successful equalization begins with good measurements of what is happening in the room. Broadband spectral trends are easily identified even in elementary real-time analyzer (RTA) measurements. Separating resonances from acoustical interference requires spatial averaging, just as it does in speaker design. For this one needs to make measurements at several different locations throughout the listening area, and they need to be averaged. This requires data acquisition and post processing, it cannot be done with several microphones plugged into a simple mixer. The measurements need to have high resolution in the frequency domain, if there is to be any hope of identifying the key parameters of problematic resonances. For this job, the RTA fails. Successful equalization requires a good equalizer. Ideally, this would be a multiple parametric-filter type. The JBL Synthesis home theater systems incorporate all of the required characteristics for successful equalization. The dedicated digital controller has 95 individually-configurable parametric filters distributed among the 5.1 channels. In the laboratory, based on high-resolution spatially-averaged anechoic measurements, some filters are preset to address small residual problems in the speakers themselves. The equalizer has helped to create better speakers. Once the system is installed in the customer's home, a trained installer arrives with a custom measurement system to adapt the system to match the room at low frequencies. The system employs five microphones connected to a multiplexer, coupled to a laptop computer which, in turn, is coupled to the digital processor. In a carefully controlled sequence, the appropriate test signals are sent through each of the channels, the measurements are compared to predetermined "target functions", and a set of filters is automatically designed that will allow the system performance to approach the target with minimum error. Built into the system are safeguards that attempt to prevent it from trying to equalize the unequalizable. Manual override is always an option, so human intervention can modify a bad decision by the computer, or accommodate customer preferences in spectral balance. At present, this is an expensive and cumbersome system. However, we are learning from our experiences in the real world. We are learning what the optimum target functions are, how best to instruct the adaptive equalization process, and what we may be able to leave out of a cost-reduced version of the system. Ideally, this should become a standard feature in popularly-priced active woofers and subwoofers. A LITTLE FOURIER ANALYSIS Before leaving this topic, let us address one of the common misunderstandings of equalization. It is frequently asserted that equalizers, because they are filters, add ringing, in the time domain. That is a fact. The assertion continues that, in addressing a frequency response problem, one may damage the transient behavior of the system. That may or may not be the case. Resonances in speakers and rooms also ring. If the filter is used to correct a resonance, both the problem and solution are minimum-phase phenomena. If the measurement of the problem has been made accurately, and the parametric filter solution has been designed to match the problem, then the ringing of the filter is equal and opposite to the ringing of the problem resonance. The ringing is gone, just as the measured peak in the frequency response has been eliminated. The real trick in this is trying to ensure that the filters are not used to "fix" something that cannot, and perhaps need not, be fixed by this means. In the days of RTA's and 1/3-octave multifilter equalizers, success of this kind was simply not possible. With today's elegant technology, it is possible, but difficult. SUBJECTIVISM vs. OBJECTIVISM - IN CONCLUSION In this lengthy summary we have covered a lot of topics. Much of it was matter-of-factly technical, driven by data and the need to measure, and much of it was subjective, driven by the desire to understand what we can hear. All of it was oriented towards creating loudspeakers that sound better. The literature of audio continues to be sprinkled with letters and articles debating the merits of science in audio. The subjectivist stance is that "to hear is to believe", and that is all that matters. Some of the arguments conjure images of white-coated engineers with putty in their ears, designing audio equipment, and not caring how it sounds, only how it measures. I have never met such a person in my 30 years in audio science and engineering. The simple fact is that, without science, there would be no audio as we know it. Without extensive and meticulous subjective evaluation, there would be no audio science as we know it. Without audio science, audio engineering reverts to trial and error. So, where does this leave us? Clearly, to be successful in this business, one must be actively involved with both of the objective and subjective sides. A faith in the scientific method is not a blind faith. It is a faith built on a growing trust that measurements can guide us to produce better sounding products at every price level, for every application. The proof, as always, is in the listening, and one MUST listen. The Harman International loudspeaker companies, JBL, Infinity, and Revel have invested heavily in measurement facilities that allow them to take the fullest advantage of existing audio science. They have invested in talented engineers who understand and respect the scientific method, good sound and great music. They have invested in elaborate listening rooms where they can enjoy and criticize the fruits of their labors. There are people on staff with many years of experience in successfully probing the frontiers of knowledge in product design and audio science, and they are equipped to continue those investigations, to push those frontiers. The arrival of multichannel audio for films required some adjustments in the performance objectives of speakers, certainly at the high end. Multichannel music is another, as yet ill-defined, challenge. More speakers in rooms, means less consumer tolerance for large boxes. Merging loudspeakers with rooms is not easy, and it is the one remaining large challenge for our industry. We are working on all of these fronts. Stay tuned. REFERENCES 1. "Listening Tests, Turning Opinion Into Fact", F.E. Toole, J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 30, pp. 431-445 (1982 June). 2. "Listening Tests - Identifying and Controlling the Variables", F.E. Toole, Proceedings of the 8th International Conference, Audio Eng, Soc. (1990 May). 3. "Subjective Evaluation", F.E. Toole, in J. Borwick, ed. "Loudspeaker and Headphone Handbook - Second Edition", chap. 11 (Focal Press, London, 1994). 4. "Subjective Measurements of Loudspeaker Sound Quality and Listener Performance", F.E. Toole, J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol 33, pp. 2-32 (1985 January/February) 5. "A Method for Training of Listeners and Selecting Program Material for Listening Tests", S. E. Olive, 97th Convention, Audio Eng. Soc., Preprint No. 3893 (1994 November). 6. "Hearing is Believing vs. Believing is Hearing: Blind vs. Sighted Listening Tests and Other Interesting Things", F.E. Toole and S.E. Olive, 97th Convention, Audio Eng. Soc., Preprint No. 3894 (1994 Nov.). 7. "Loudspeaker Measurements and Their Relationship to Listener Preferences", F.E. Toole, J. Audio Eng, Soc., vol. 34, pt.1 pp.227-235 (1986 April), pt. 2, pp. 323-348 (1986 May). 8. "Loudspeakers and Rooms for Stereophonic Sound Reproduction", F.E. Toole, Proceedings of the 8th International Conference, Audio Eng, Soc. (1990 May). 9. "The Modification of Timbre by Resonances: Perception and Measurement", F.E. Toole and S.E. Olive, J. Audio Eng, Soc., vol. 36, pp. 122-142 (1988 March). 10. "The Detection of Reflections in Typical Rooms", S.E. Olive and F.E. Toole, J. Audio Eng, Soc., vol. 37, pp. 539-553 (1989 July/August). 11. "The Future of Stereo", F.E. Toole, Part 1, Audio, vol 81, no.5, pp. 126-142 (1997 May), Part 2, Audio, vol.8, no.6, pp. 34-39 (1997 June). 12. "Perception of Reproduced Sound in Rooms: Some Results from the Athena Project", P. L. Schuck, S. Olive, J. Ryan, F. E. Toole, S Sally, M. Bonneville, E. Verreault, Kathy Momtahan, pp.49-73, Proceedings of the 12th International Conference, Audio Eng. Soc. (1993 June). 13. "The Detection Thresholds of Resonances at Low Frequencies", S.E. Olive, P. Schuck, J. Ryan, S. Sally, M. Bonneville, J. Audio Eng. Soc. Vol. 45, No. 3 (1997 March.) 14. "The Variability of Loudspeaker Sound Quality Among Four Domestic-Sized Rooms", S.E. Olive, P. Schuck, J. Ryan, S. Sally, M. Bonneville, presented at the 99th AES Convention, preprint 4092 K-1 (1995 October). 15. "The Effects of Loudspeaker Placement on Listener Preference Ratings", S.E. Olive, P. Schuck, S. Sally, M. Bonneville, J. Audio Eng. Soc., Vol. 42, pp. 651-669 (1994 September) 5/8/98 rev. 8/19/99 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 A New Laboratory for Evaluating Multichannel Audio Components and Systems SEAN E. OLIVE, AES Fellow, BRIAN CASTRO, AES Member, AND FLOYD E. TOOLE, AES Fellow R&D Group, Harman International Industries Inc., 8500 Balboa Blvd., Northridge, CA, 91329, USA Email: ABSTRACT The design criteria, features and acoustic measurements of a new listening laboratory designed specifically for listening tests on multichannel loudspeakers and components are described. Among its features is a novel automated speaker shuffler that eliminates loudspeaker position effects or allows the variable to be efficiently tested. Other features include complete computer control of experimental design, control and collection of listener data, making listening tests more reliable and efficient. 1.0 INTRODUCTION Listening tests are the final arbiter for determining whether an audio product sounds good, and they play a critical role in the research and development of new products. Designing and conducting listening tests that produce reliable and accurate data is, however, no simple task. There are many variables other than those under test that unless removed or controlled can seriously bias the results [1-9]. Two of the more difficult variables to control are the listening room [5],[7],[9] and the position(s) of the loudspeakers under test [5],[9] both of which can significantly influence the sounds that arrive at listeners' ears and listeners' perceptions of them. Recently we had the opportunity to design and construct a new state-of-the-art listening laboratory to be used for developing and subjectively testing multichannel loudspeakers and other components. The goal from the outset was to build and equip a listening laboratory that could generate subjective measurements as accurate, efficient and free of 1 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 bias as possible. To meet these goals, a large effort went into developing hardware and software that would automate the design and control of experiments, including the collection, storage and statistical analysis of listener data. Included in the design is a novel automated speaker shuffler that performs positional substitution of 9 loudspeakers so that positional biases can be eliminated or efficiently tested. By eliminating position as a variable, the speaker shuffler has reduced the length of a typical multiple loudspeaker listening test by a factor of 24:1 making product development faster and less costly. Another notable feature of the room is that the acoustics can be easily varied from almost hemi-anechoic to semi-reverberant by adding removable reflective panels to the walls and ceiling. This paper describes the rationale, features and measurements of the new listening facility, which we call the Multichannel Listening Laboratory (MLL). Finally, the results are compared with several current international standards that recommend performance criteria for listening rooms intended for critical listening. 1.1 Listening Room Standards Several standards recommend values for various acoustic parameters that define listening room performance. The goal of these standards is to facilitate the replication of listening evaluations in different rooms under the same test conditions. This is particularly important for radio and television broadcast corporations, audio production facilities, large audio equipment manufacturers, and international standards and research organizations, all of whom have multiple facilities in which critical judgments are made on the same program material or equipment. Ideally, if the listening rooms and test conditions in which these judgments are made are sufficiently similar, and the listeners have normal hearing are properly trained, then a consensus in opinion should be possible. If not, then there is likely something wrong with the test procedure itself. In reviewing these various standards, a serious problem common to many is that while they define tolerances for specific acoustic parameters, they do not adequately define how the parameter is to be measured. For example, IEC is the only standard that specifies how reverberation time should be measured, even though it has been shown that RT60 can vary widely depending on the technique used. Unfortunately, this rather defeats the purpose of defining a standard in the first place! It is conceivable that one measurement method may show the room meets the standard, while another measurement method may not. Added to this is the belief, held by some authorities, that in small rooms, reverberation time is a parameter of little or no value. A very good discussion and summary of standards as they relate to the design of multichannel listening room intended for loudspeaker listening tests are given by Jarvinen et al in [9]. The current standards that recommend listening room performance include: 2 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 1. IEC Publication 268-13: Sound System Equipment, part 13. Listening Tests on Loudspeakers (1985) [10 ] 2. NR-12 A, Technical Recommendation: Sound Control Rooms and Listening Rooms. 2nd Edition, The Nordic Public Broadcasting Corporation, (1992) [11] 3. ITU-R Recommendation BS.1116: Methods for Subjective Evaluation of Small Impairments in audio systems including multichannel sound systems, 2nd Edition (1997) [12] 4. ITU-R Recommendation BS.775: Multichannel stereophonic sound system with and without accompanying picture (1994) [13] 5. EBU Tech 3276 (2nd Edition, 1997 ) [ 14] 6. AES20-1996: Recommended Practice for Professional Audio - Subjective Evaluation of Loudspeakers (1996) [15 ] The standards can be classified according to the intended application of the listening room and can be generally classified into two groups. The AES and IEC standards were intended for monophonic and stereophonic testing of loudspeakers in typical domestic listening rooms. Both these standards are now quite old and the recommended room sizes are too small to allow multiple comparison of multichannel systems. The EBU, ITU and NR standards were drafted primarily by broadcasters and allow for much larger control rooms that can accommodate several listeners at a time. Only the AES, IEC and ITU standards include recommendations for listening test methodology. At the design stage, we did not intentionally set out to meet any of the above standards. However, in post-hoc examination have found that our listening room meets both ITU and EBU standards in its current configuration in which we have added reflective and diffractive surfaces to both the ceiling and walls. In the following sections we show measurements made in the MLL and compare these with various acoustical properties recommended in the above standards. These properties include dimensions, floor area, volume, proportions, reverberation time and background noise. The values measured for the MLL are compared with the recommended values in Table 1 for each standard, and shows that the MLL meets both ITU and EBU recommendations. 2.0 MULTICHANNEL LISTENING LAB (MLL) 2.1 Room Dimensions The listening room itself consists of double-wall constructed shell built by Industrial Acoustics Corporation (IAC). The dimensions of the MLL were largely dictated by our requirements to be able to evaluate up to 3 different 5.1 or 7.1 channel systems at a time and accommodate 1-6 listeners. The room also had to be sufficiently large to accommodate our automated 9-loudspeaker shuffler that requires a space of approximately 9 m (L) x 1.5 m (W) x 1 m (D). This resulted the following dimensions: 3 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 MLL Dimensions Length 9.14 m Width 6.58 m Height 2.59 m Floor Area 60.20 m 2 Volume 155.92 m 3 As shown in Table 1, the MLL satisfies the recommended volume and floor area values specified in ITU, EBU, and N12 standards. The MLL's volume exceeds the IEC and AES recommended limits of 110 m 3 and 120 m 3 respectively because the standards were intended for small domestic stereo listening rooms. 2.2 Room Proportions The most problematic performance issue in small listening rooms is non-uniform low frequency reproduction caused by standing waves that produce large pressure peaks and nulls in the lower 3-4 octaves of the audio range. The distribution and frequencies at which these peaks and notches occur are directly related to the geometry of the room. If the ratio of the room dimensions is carefully chosen, a more uniform response is possible. Walker from the BBC [16] has created a room geometry criterion that has been adopted by both the EBU and ITU standards. The "Walker" criterion defines the limits of the ratios for length (l), width (w) and height (h) as: 1.1 w = l = 5.4 w -4 (1) hh h As shown in Table 1, the ratio of dimensions for the MLL meet the "Walker" criterion and therefore satisfies the EBU and ITU standards. The relatively large size of the MLL also benefit uniform frequency response in the lower octaves since the first order width and length modes are below 25 Hz. 2.3 Background Noise Accurate and repeatable subjective measurements require a listening room with low background noise so those listeners are able to reliably judge the quality of low-level signals. Perception of timbre, nonlinear distortion, loudness and spatial qualities are all influenced by the presence and masking effects of background noise. Minimizing background noise in the MLL was carefully considered during the design and construction. The IAC double-wall shell itself is located in a large room that has limited access to both people and noisy equipment. No part of the shell touches the structural walls of the building except the floor, which is mechanically floated. 4 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 The inner walls and ceiling of the double-wall IAC shell are made of heavy gauge steel panels separated 10 cm and filled with fiberglass. The inner surfaces are perforated with 2.34-mm openings to provide substantial sound absorption inside the room. The inner walls are entirely floated and separated from the outer wall of the shell by a 10 cm space to minimize mechanical and acoustic transmission of noise. The room has its own dedicated HVAC system with ventilation silencers and acoustically lined ducts that create a comfortable and quiet environment. For experiments that require extremely low background noise the room can be cooled and the HVAC can be completely shut off during the test. The room requires minimal lighting during the test itself (i.e. 1 Halogen light) which means that noise from lights is not an issue. All audio equipment, other than the required amplifiers, is located outside the room, and this also helps to minimize electrical noise as well. In an effort to simulate the construction of floors found in many homes, a carpeted "squeak-free" plywood floor was laid on 5 cm x 15 cm wooden joists separated 41 cm apart. The joists are mounted on 6.4 mm neoprene pads for isolation from the concrete floor beneath. The rationale for constructing this floor is to allow transmission of low bass from the loudspeaker through the floor to the listeners' feet, since the perception of bass depends on what is felt, as well as what is heard. The front and middle sections of the floor can be removed to allow easy access of audio, video and data cables that run underneath the floor to access panels both inside and outside the room. In reviewing the various listening room standards there is a wide range of recommended levels for background noise. The most stringent requirements are specified by the EBU and ITU standards, which call for minimum level of NR10, not exceeding NR15. These rather demanding requirements are likely justified in broadcast environments where listeners are frequently required to evaluate small signal linearity, for example in relation to CODECS. At the other extreme, the AES and IEC standards both have rather liberal recommended background noise limit of 35 dBA measured using a slow time constant. The AES standard has an additional limit of 50 dB C-weighted for low frequency noise. The less stringent requirements are likely justified on the basis that they are aimed at loudspeaker evaluations in typical domestic environments where background noise levels are typically higher. Figure 1 shows the background noise measured in the MLL with the air conditioner turned both on and off. Also plotted are the NR curves 0 through 15. The MLL noise curves each represent an average of four measurements take at 4 different locations around the listening area. The time over which each measurement was averaged was 64 s. The measurement was taken using a Bruel & Kjaer 4179, 1 inch microphone, a Bruel & Kjaer preamp Type 2660, and a Bruel & Kjaer real-time analyzer. The low noise microphone and preamp allow accurate measurement of sound pressure levels below the threshold of hearing, which is necessary at higher frequencies for measuring rooms below NR20. Figure 1 shows that with the air conditioning turned off, the MLL meets NR5, 5 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 thus meeting the requirements of the EBU and ITU specification. With the air conditioning turned on the noise increases to NR15. 2.4 Reverberation Time The reflected sounds and reverberation time in a room have been shown to have an important influence on the perception of loudness, timbre and spatial qualities and speech intelligibility in both live and reproduced sound. While this is a complex phenomena, the acoustic community sees fit to summarize it all in a T60 measurement. Both the EBU and the ITU standards specify values for the average reverberation time in the room. ITU and EBU recommend the value (within a tolerance of ± 0.05 s) be determined using the following equation: 1/3 25.0 V T = m s (2) V ref where Tm is the average reverberation time between 200 Hz to 4 kHz, V is the volume of the room, and Vref is the reference volume of 100 3 m. The EBU also put limits on the range of values specifying that the value should lie between 0.2 Tm 0.4 s. The IEC standard specifies a Tm of 0.3 - 0.6 seconds which is very similar to the AES standard that recommends 0.45 s ( ±0.05 s). The N 12-A standard specifies Tm be measured in 1/3 octaves between 200 Hz to 2.5 kHz and be determined as a function of the floor area using the following equation: 35.0 S T = m ±s 05.0 (3) S ref where S is the floor area of the room and S ref is the reference area of 60 2 m. In addition to specifying the average reverberation time, most of the standards recommend that Tm be relatively independent of frequency within a certain bandwidth and tolerance. For ITU and EBU standards, the Tm value for each octave band between 200 Hz - 3.5 kHz should vary no more than ±0.05 s from the calculated optimum value. Below 200 Hz, Tm is allowed to increase monotonically with frequency to 0.3 s above the optimum value. Above 3.5 kHz, the tolerance is increased to ±0.1 s from the optimal value. By substituting the volume of the MLL (155.92 3 m) into equation (2), we calculate that Tm should be 0.29 s to meet ITU and EBU standards. According to N 12-A, the Tm for the MLL should be 0.35 s. 6 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 The Tm of the MLL was measured using a MLSSA system from DRA laboratory. The microphone was a Bruel & Kjaer 4134 microphone. The sound source consisted of four JBL Synthesis satellite loudspeakers crossed at 80 Hz over to a JBL Synthesis Two subwoofer located in the corner of the room. Each of the four satellites was located approximately 2 m apart and aimed at a different corner in an attempt to create a diffuse sound field. The measurement shown in Figure 2 represents a spatial average of four microphone locations. The average Tm value for the MLL is about 0.23 s, which is slightly below the calculated ITU and EBU optimal value of 0.29 s. However, the curve falls within the minimum recommended value, and is quite uniform with frequency, only rising slightly below 125 Hz. 2.5 Control of Early Reflections With the advent of 5.1 and 7.1 multichannel and 3D audio playback systems, there is a trend among professional and home theater listening room designs towards lower reverberation times and the control of early reflections. There are sound scientific reasons for doing this, since strong early reflections are known to influence the perceived spatial and timbral qualities of reproduced sound [7], [17]. In the new generation of multichannel recordings and video disks, the additional center and surround channels allow the producer and recording artist to create much more realistic and spatiallyenriched environments than ever before. There is less need to use the room's boundaries and the loudspeakers' directional characteristics to compensate for the obvious spatial deficiencies inherent to stereo. The EBU standard recommends that all reflections within the first 15 ms after the arrival of sound be no greater than 10 dB in level relative to the direct sound from each sound source. With multichannel setups the early sound field is rather complex given that there are between 5-7 loudspeakers and several boundaries. For example with 5 loudspeakers and 6 boundaries there are 30 first order reflections and 150 second order reflections. Measuring and separating out these reflections is no trivial task. The reflections from the floor are particularly problematic to treat since in most facilities, the floor surfaces must be hard and reflective to facilitate the movement of people and equipment. Nonetheless, several organizations [18], [19] are building such rooms that meet this reflection-free part of the specification with the exception of the floor bounce. In the MLL room, the only significant first order reflections are from the floor, and these are attenuated at higher frequencies by the carpet. At listener-loudspeaker distances greater than 2 m any reflection with a path length greater than 6.34 m will be attenuated 10 dB by spreading loss [18]. This effectively eliminates all second order reflections since their path length exceeds this value. For front channel sources, first order reflections from the side walls will also be sufficiently delayed beyond the 15 ms time gap. The main culprits are reflections from the front and back walls, and the ceiling. Fortunately these surfaces can be made absorptive by simply removing the reflective panels so that the absorptive surface is exposed. To reduce flutter echoes from reflective surfaces and to increase reverberation, 120 RPG Skylines, an omnidirectional primitive root number theory 2D diffusor, are placed on the reflective panels located on the walls, 7 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 as well as on the ceiling and areas behind the loudspeaker as shown in Figures 4 and 5. These light-weight diffusors are easily removed or relocated, and help reduce any other specular reflections that may arrive after the direct sound. 2.6 Automated Speaker Mover The position of a loudspeaker in a room has a significant impact on its perceived sound quality. Changing its position affects the way it couples to the standing wave modes of the room, and alters the physical characteristics of broadband reflections that arrive at the listener. In listening tests that involve multiple comparisons among loudspeakers the positional effects on listeners' ratings can be larger than the differences between the loudspeakers under test [8]. Unless these positional effects are controlled, the results may be contaminated by a nuisance variable. For multiple comparison loudspeaker tests, asking human beings to sit behind a doubleblind screen and quickly and smoothly substitute the positions of 2-9 loudspeakers (some weighing upwards to 100 kg) on command presents an obvious logistical problem. Clearly the problem of positional substitution calls for an automated solution. This realization led to the development of our own custom-built speaker shuffler. Prior to having a speaker shuffler, the positional effects in loudspeaker tests had been balanced by testing each loudspeaker in each position. Any position-related bias would be equally distributed or balanced across each loudspeaker. More scientifically rigorous designs go even further and test all possible loudspeaker-position permutations so that any possible context effects between loudspeaker and position are also balanced. The disadvantage of not having a speaker mover is that an additional number of trials are required to balance the variable position. This relationship in illustrated in Figures 3(a)- (b), which compare the number of trials required to balance the variable position in multiple comparison tests, with and without a speaker mover. The number of trials is calculated using the following equation: Trials of Number = N Positions Speaker ! × N Programs × N Repeats (4) Where N Speaker Positions equals the number of speaker positions in the test, N Programs equals the number of program selections being used and N Repeats is the number of repeats. In Figure 3 we, the experimental design shows no repeats, that is N Repeat = 1. The graphs clearly shows that an automated speaker mover can drastically reduce the length of the experiment because the variable N Speaker Positions always equals 1, regardless of how many loudspeakers are compared. In comparing the two graphs we see that there is a 2:1 advantage for paired comparisons, a 6:1 advantage for triple comparisons, and a 24:1 advantage for comparisons among four loudspeakers. When you multiply these ratios by the number of programs and repeats used in the experimental design, the number of trials quickly escalates. For multiple comparisons between four loudspeakers using 4 programs with no repeats, a total of 96 trials are required without a speaker mover. Having a speaker mover reduces the experiment to 4 trials. This enormous 8 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 difference provided the justification to design and build a custom speaker shuffler, since over the long-term, it could afford considerable savings in person-listening hours and product development time. A custom-built floor at the front of the room allows us to perform positional substitution of up to 9 different loudspeakers. A photograph of the speaker mover set up for an A/B stereo loudspeaker comparison is shown in Figure 4. Figure 5 shows a photograph of the speaker mover set up for a single comparison of a 5.1 loudspeaker system. For the purposes of the photograph the front, side and rear listening curtains have been retraced out of the way. Each loudspeaker is attached to one of nine pallets that move in 1-inch increments over a range of 4 feet forwards and backwards while the entire array moves 4 feet to the left and right of the listener. The movement of the floor can be controlled manually from a programmable logic controller (PLC), or from a computer that is linked serially to the PLC via RS232. This allows all positions of loudspeakers to be programmed, stored and recalled quickly. The movement of the floor is extremely quiet, repeatable to within 1 inch, and fast. Transit time between positions is no greater than 3 s, and most positional changes are under 2 s. The transit speed is also programmable and can be decreased or increased if desired. As a safety measure, a light fence is installed in front of the moving floor so that if anyone crosses the light beam the speaker mover automatically stops. The speaker shuffler allows position-controlled loudspeaker comparisons in mono (up to 4 different systems), stereo (4 different systems) or three different left/center/right channel loudspeakers. At this time, positional substitution of surround and rear channel speakers must be done manually for multichannel experiments. The speakers can be placed away from the side and rear boundaries on stands, or placed on adjustable shelves that are mounted on baffles made of high-density board, that slide in a track along the perimeter of the room. The moving floor gives us an efficient means to eliminate the effects of loudspeaker position, or it can do the reverse, and allow us to test the interaction effects between loudspeaker and position. By statistically-averaging a loudspeaker's performance over a number of different positions we can assess its off-axis performance, and a number of other parameters that are position dependent. All of this becomes essential as we aim to design loudspeakers that are 'room friendly' and develop digital room equalization systems. Finally, the speaker mover also allows us to efficiently randomize between each trial, how the loudspeaker is identified to the listener (e.g. "A,B,C..). This ensures that listeners' judgments in each trial are statistically independent between program selections. Without a speaker mover, experimenters normally do not move the loudspeakers behind the screen until a complete block of programs has been rated. These are not independent judgments since the listener knows they are rating the same loudspeaker(s) within each block. The extent to which this biases the results has not yet been reported. 9 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 2.7 Blind versus Sighted Listening Tests It is generally accepted among scientists that psychometric experiments must be performed double blind. For audio tests, this means the identities of the components under test cannot be made known to the listener, and the experimenter cannot not directly control or administer the actual test. In 1996 Toole and Olive in [2] conducted some blind versus sighted loudspeaker tests that showed both experienced and inexperienced listeners' judgments were significantly influenced by factors such as price, brand name, size and cosmetics. In fact, the effect of these biases in the sighted tests were larger than any other significant factors found in the blind tests, including loudspeaker, position and program interactions. These experiments clearly show that an accurate and unbiased measurement of sound quality requires that the tests be done blind. To remove these biases from listening tests in the MLL an acoustically transparent curtain that is visually opaque is placed between the products and the listeners so that they do not know the identities of the products under test. All other associated equipment in the signal path is also out-of-sight and locked in an equipment rack, since the performance and paranoia of some listeners can be affected by simply having knowledge that a certain brand of interconnect or CD player is in the signal path. The front screen consists of a black open knit polyester knit cloth chosen for its acoustic transparency and used as grille clothe in many of our loudspeakers. The material is attached to a large automated curtain roller so it can be easily lifted down and up with an infrared remote control. Weights are attached to a seam in the bottom so the cloth retains its tautness when in use. Retractable curtains made of the same material surround the listeners to hide the identities of loudspeakers located at the sides and rear of the listening room. Figures 4 and 5 show the front, side and rear curtains fully retracted when not in use, and Figure 8 shows the curtains in place during an actual listening test. 2.8 Video Playback Video and audio are increasingly becoming recorded, processed and distributed together. There is a growing interest among researchers in studying how the perceived quality of one affects the perception of the other. Although much research still needs to be done, evidence suggests there are bimodal interactions between the two that influence listeners' expectations and judgments of the quality of the audio, and vice versa. Keeping this in mind, we were careful in selecting a video playback system within our budget that had sufficient quality, so that it would not negatively impact listeners' opinions of the sound quality. We selected a three gun front projection CRT made by Audio Video Source for its above- average picture quality and the additional advantage that is has no fan. The picture is projected on a 100 inch Stewart Microperf screen that is retractable so it can be removed 10 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 for audio-only listening tests. The acoustical effect of the screen is another factor that is not completely understood, and will be a subject of investigation. 2.9 Automated Control, Collection and Analysis of Data In designing the MLL, we wanted to automate as much as possible the design and running of experiments including the collection, storage and analysis of data, in order to reduce the time and costs of performing listening tests. Automation of experiments has the additional benefit of making listening tests more reproducible, largely because it reduces the risk of human errors and biases introduced by the experimenter. Considerable ongoing effort in software development is helping us to fulfill these goals. Automation begins at the experimental design stage where all important experimental parameters and details are defined by the experimenter as a "*.exp" file that is stored in a database that resides on the Windows NT server. The experiment file contains the following information: .. The name of the experiment and a brief description .. Detailed information related to the experimental design and protocol including definition of scales and randomization of variables. Protocol choices include single or multiple comparisons, ABX, ABC(with hidden reference) and different threshold measurement protocols. .. Instructions to the listeners .. Equipment control information and operational parameters required by the audio switcher for level matching, switching and overall output level. .. The file names or track information for each program selection. This information is sent to the appropriate signal source device. .. Information related to the position and movement of loudspeakers .. A list of trials which the software randomly selects The Windows NT server controls the running of the experiment including control of all associated equipment in the signal path. A block diagram of the equipment and signal path for the MLL is shown in Appendix 1. The lines that connect each block as well as the signal paths are color coded and typed according to whether the signals are audio (either analog or digital), video, infrared or RF control, computer data, MIDI control or sent over PCI or serial buss. The signal sources are the blocks on the top left of Appendix 1. They currently include DVD and Laser Disk player, an 8-channel PCM digital recorder, and an 8-channel PC-based hard disk recorder (Lexicon Studio) and its associated A/D and D/A I/O cards. The audio and video outputs of the DVD and LD players are sent to the Lexicon DC-1 which provides AC-3 and DTS decoding when required. The analog outputs are sent to the Spirit 328 digital mixer which provides signal switching and level matching (within 0.03 dB) for up to 16 analog or digital inputs. The 8 channel sources are sent digitally to the Spirit mixer and remain digital up to the power amplifier before they are converted by the Studer D/A's. All operational parameters of the Spirit mixer can be viewed, stored and recalled from the NT Server via MIDI control. 11 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 The input of listener data, feedback and status information is done using laptops connected to the NT Server through a LAN. For single listener experiments, the listener can control switching of the stimulae remotely from their laptop. A photograph of a listener entering data on the laptop connected to the NT Server is shown in Figure 6. For multiple listener experiments, the NT Server controls the switching either manually or through software automation. During the experiment, all changes in listener response data can be viewed in real-time on the NT Server which performs running statistical averages and graphs of the results. Remote access to the NT Server and control of the equipment from inside the listening room is also possible through a wireless RF mouse, keyboard and a flat panel display, all of which are connected to the Server. This might be required during set up or for informal listening sessions or product demonstrations. The flat panel display also shows status information to the listener(s) indicating what stimulus (i.e. A, B, C.) is currently selected, and any other necessary information. Finally, all experimental data and information related to listeners (date and time, name, seat position, age) is stored in a relational data base which can be formatted and imported into various statistical packages we use for analysis of results. Not shown in the block diagram is a video camera used for monitoring subjects and to detect and hopefully deter possible cheating. Also not shown is a two-way intercom that allows communication between the subject(s) and the experimenter. 3.0 CONTROL ROOM AND LISTENER TRAINING LAB Outside the MLL is a lab area dedicated for audio and test equipment used during the set up, running and monitoring of listening experiments. Here a space is also dedicated for the training of listeners, which is done over headphones at computer audio workstations. Bech in [20] has shown that 6 trained listeners can provide data that is as statistically reliable as data gathered from 18 untrained listeners. Clearly, considerable cost-savings in time and money can be realized if listeners are trained before they participate in formal listening experiments. At Harman, listeners with normal hearing undergo a listener training program, which self-administered through a computer and custom software developed in-house [21]. The software teaches listeners to identify and rate using different scales, frequency response irregularities according to the center frequency, amplitude and Q of the distortion. The graphical user interface of the training software is shown in Figure 8. The training focuses on frequency-related problems since these are the common and most serious audible problems found in most loudspeaker-related listening tests, which many untrained listeners find difficult to describe. The training solves this problem by teaching 12 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 listeners to describe these phenomena in technical terms that design engineers can understand and use to correct any problematic audible artifacts in product designs. The training software has proved to be a valuable tool for teaching listeners how to describe and scale the various dimensions of sound quality in meaningful terms, and allows their performance to be quantified in terms that allow us to discriminate good listeners from bad ones. An additional, indirect, benefit accrued from training is that we have learned which program selections are most revealing of typical frequency-related artifacts introduced during the training exercises, and we now use these in our product evaluations. 4.0 CONCLUSIONS In summary, we have described a new facility designed to test multichannel components efficiently and as bias-free as possible. The facility includes acoustically transparent listening screens that hide the identities of all multichannel loudspeakers and equipment within the audio path. Particular attention has been taken to address the two of the most problematic variables in listening tests: the listening room and the position(s) of the loudspeaker. Through the use of a computer automated speaker shuffler, we have greatly reduced the amount of time and effort required to set up and test multiple comparisons between loudspeakers by reducing the factor position to a one-dimension or level variable. Typical loudspeaker evaluations should be reduced in length by a factor of 24:1. The listening room itself is capable of testing up to three different 5.1 or 7.1 channel systems and accommodate 1-6 listeners at a time. The measurements we have shown in this paper indicate its performance in its current form meets the very highest standards set out by the ITU and EBU recommendations, in terms of volume, geometry, reverberation time, and the control of early reflections. The acoustics of the room can be easily altered from hemi-anechoic to more typical domestic room conditions by adding reflective panels to the room's boundaries. Finally, the experimental design, set up and control are computer-automated so that experiments can be easily repeated, and are less prone to human error. The more timeconsuming and mundane tasks such as collection and analysis of data have also been computer-automated, so that experiment report writing becomes a simple cut-and-paste operation. 5.0 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors would like to thank Tom Roberts of Bruel & Kjaer for his assistance and loan of the equipment used to make the background noise measurements shown in this paper. 13 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 6.0 REFERENCES [1] F.E. Toole, "Listening Tests - Identifying and Controlling the Variables", Proceedings of the 8th International Conference, Audio Eng., Soc. (1990 May). [2] F.E. Toole and S.E. Olive, "Hearing is Believing vs. Believing is Hearing: Blind vs. Sighted Listening Tests and Other Interesting Things", 97th Convention, Audio Eng. Soc., Preprint No. 3894 (1994 Nov.) [3] F.E. Toole, "Listening Tests, Turning Opinion Into Fact", J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 30, pp. 431-445 (1982 June). [4] F.E. Toole, "Subjective Measurements of Loudspeaker Sound Quality and Listener Performance", J. Audio Eng. Soc., vol. 33, pp. 2-32 (1985 January/February). [5] Soren Bech, " Perception of Timbre of Reproduced Sound in Small Rooms: Influence of Room and Loudspeaker Position J AES, Vol. 42, Number 12 pp. 999 (1994). [6] S.E. Olive, P. Schuck, J. Ryan, S. Sally, M. Bonneville, "The Variability of Loudspeaker Sound Quality Among Four Domestic-Sized Rooms", presented at the 99th AES Convention, preprint 4092 K-1 (1995 October). [7] F.E. Toole, "Loudspeakers and Rooms for Stereophonic Sound Reproduction", Proceedings of the 8th International Conference, Audio Eng., Soc. (1990 May). [8] S.E. Olive, P. Schuck, S. Sally, M. Bonneville, "The Effects of Loudspeaker Placement on Listener Preference Ratings", J. Audio Eng. Soc., Vol. 42, pp. 651-669 (1994 September). [9] Antti Jarvinen, Lauri Savioja, Henrik Moller, Veijo Ikonen, Anssi Ruusuvuori, "Design of a Reference Listening Room - A Case Study", AES 103rd Convention, New York, Preprint 4559, September 26-29, 1997. [10] IEC Publication 268-13: Sound System Equipment, part 13. Listening Tests on Loudspeakers (1985) [11 NR-12 A, Technical Recommendation: Sound Control Rooms and Listening Rooms. 2nd Edition, The Nordic Public Broadcasting Corporation, 1992. [12] ITU-R Recommendation BS.1116: Methods for Subjective Evaluation of Small Impairments in audio systems including multichannel sound systems, 2nd Edition (1997) [13] ITU-R Recommendation BS.775: Multichannel stereophonic sound system with and without accompanying picture (1994). [14] EBU Tech 3276 (2nd Edition, 1997). [15] AES20-1996: Recommended Practice for Professional Audio - Subjective Evaluation of Loudspeakers (1996). [16] Walker, R. "Optimum Dimension Ratios For Small Rooms". 100th AES Convention. Preprint 4191 (Copenhagen, Denmark, 1996). [17] S.E. Olive and F.E. Toole, "The Detection of Reflections in Typical Rooms", J. Audio Eng., Soc., vol. 37, pp. 539-553 (1989 July/August). [18] R.Walker," A controlled-reflection listening room for multichannel sound", AES 104th Convention Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Preprint #4645, May 16-19, 1998 14 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 [19] E. Arató Borsi, T. Póth, and A. Fürjes," New Reference Listening Room for Two- Channel and Multichannel Stereophonic" AES 104th Convention Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Preprint #4732, May 16-19, 1998. [20]Soren Bech,"Selection and Training of Subjects for Listening Tests on Sound- Reproducing Equipment" Vol. 40, Number 7 pp. 590 (1992). [21] S. E. Olive, "A Method for Training of Listeners and Selecting Program Material for Listening Tests", 97th Convention, Audio Eng. Soc., Preprint No. 3893 (1994 November). TABLE 1 Parameter Harman ITU EBU N12-A IEC AES MLL Volume 155.92 60-110 50-120 ( m 3 ) (80) Floor area 60.20 20-70 40 60 ± 10 20 ( m 2 ) Height 2.59 2.3 - 3.0 rec. 2.1 h (m) 2.8 Length 9.14 = 6 l (m) rec. 6.7 Width 6.58 = 4 w (m) rec. 4.2 (1.1 w / h) 2.80 ( l / h) 3.53 ( 4.5w / h - 4 ) 7.44 T m (s) 0.23 0.29 0.29 0.35 0.3 -0.6 0.45 ± 0.15 ± 0.05 0.4 ± 0.05 T 63 Hz Max .34 Tm(s) 0.2 - 0.4 0.35 0.8 (s) Noise Level NR 5 NR10; NR10;NR 10 L pA L pA 35 dB abs. max abs. max or L pA 35 dB andNR 15 NR 15 15 dB L pC 50 dB Table 1: Dimensions and Acoustic Parameters of Harman MLL versus Recommendations of Various Standards 15 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 32 63 125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 ) SPL ( ) -10 Frequency (Hz dBAC ON AC OFF NR0 NR5 NR10 NR15 Figure 1 A spatially-averaged measurement showing the background noise in the MLL with the air conditioning off (dotted) and turned on (dashed) compared to the NR curves: 0,5,10 and 15. 0 63 250 500 2000 () T60 (seconds) in 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 125 1000 4000 8000 FrequencyHzEBU & ITU OPT. EBU & ITU Max EBU & ITU MMLL Figure 2 The Tm (RT60) values measured in the MLL compared to the optimal, maximum and minimum values recommended by the EBU and ITU standards. 16 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 () 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 1 2 3 4 /o /o /o /o Minimum Number of Trials Without Speaker MoverNumber of Loudspeaker Positions Compared 1 program w2 program s w3 program s w4 program s w Figure 3(A) The above graph shows the number of trials required for a multiple comparison loudspeaker experiment as a function of the number loudspeaker positions compared. The lines represent experiments in which 1-4 programs are used. The design balances all position and context effects and has no repeats. () 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 1 2 3 4 Minimum Number of Trials With Speaker MoverNumber of Loudspeaker Positions Compared 1 program w. 2 programs w. 3 programs w. 4 programs w. Figure 3(B) The same experiment is shown as in Figure 3(A) above except here a speaker mover is used. 17 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 Figure 4 Shown is the automated speaker shuffler of the MLL set up for A/B stereo testing of two stereo loudspeakers. Here the front listening screen is pulled up. Figure 5 A front-left wide-angle shot of the MLL with the listening screens pulled back. The automated speaker shuffler is in the foreground setup for 5.1 playback. Note the side and rear channel speaker baffles in the background, and the audio and computer data control box on the back wall. The video projector is mounted on the ceiling with a retractable screen in front of the speaker mover. 18 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 Figure 6 A listener performing a test by entering their data on a laptop computer that is networked to the NT Server. In this test, video is displayed and both front, side and rear curtains are drawn to hide the identifies of the 5.1 loudspeaker systems under test. Figure 7 Shown is the control room area outside the listening room where all audio equipment,experimental control and monitoring takes place. Shown here is the NT Server on the left, and two listener training workstations on the right. 19 Harman International Industries, Incorporated 8500 Balboa Blvd., PO Box 2200, Northridge, CA 91329 (818) 893-8411 Figure 8: The GUI of the listener training software. The listeners' task is to match the 4 different equalizations indicates by their frequency response curves that are randomly assigned to Buttons A-D Feedback is given on their responses. The "FLAT" button allows listeners to audition the program without any equalization added. Figure 9: The GUI of the software used for a typical listening test or training exercise. Listeners enter their preference ratings for sounds A-D relative to a given reference ("REF"). Ratings are also given on spectral balance and distortion. Relevant comments are optional. 20 Appendix 1: Block Diagram of Harman Multichannel Listening Laboratory (MLL) showing key features, equipment and path for audio, video, data and control signals. Speaker Mover ateProceed (Controlled via Amplifier RS-232) (16 channels) 2200, N Studer 8411 D/A Converter (16 channels) Sony PCM800 Curtain Stewart DVD Video Screen Flat Panel Display Laser Disk Wireless Infrared Analog Audio Mouse + Digital Audio Keyboard Lexicon DC-1 Video Listeners' Computer Data MIDI Data Laptops Computer Card Spirit Digital 328 Faroudja Lexicon Front Line I/O Box Projector Doubler MIDI Card Ethernet WINDOWS HUB NT SERVER Lexicon Studio Core PCI Card RS232/IR /RF 21 Controller |
#2
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. |
#3
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. If you don't like what I write, don't read it. |
#4
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. |
#5
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast
[dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. |
#6
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 00:53:45 GMT, wrote:
"George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. I've got a modern newsreader that has the number of lines listed right next to the subject in the message list pane (and George has that same newreader). You should try such a newsreader sometime. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. |
#7
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() Arny Krueger wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Nice to hear from you, Arny. I've been looking for your contribution to the "The case for ABX..." thread. I thought you might be unwell or on leave but I see that you let your flock take the grapeshot. Well an ArchPriest has to save his irreplaceable self. Your answer to Middius allows two interpretations: 1) you do not understand that "posturing" refers to Nyob's appropriating a serious article 2) You're up to your debating trickery which I continue to think is not worthy of you. As for Ludovic needing lessons from fatuous NYOB in the value of double-blind testing{ For the umpteenth time:1) I was doing DBT medical research before any of you heard the name. Medical DBT research can be validated by objective data (psyche is an exception) 2) DBT is DBT, ABX is ABX 3) I use blind technique sometimes myself when I listen to a new component. Not yours but mine Left-Right method.4) But when it comes to invalidable preferences in musical reproduction I'll take eg. Holt's opinion sighted any time over NYOBs nth power seventy times blind. 5) Research is another matter entirely: double blind provides uniformity. I need no convincing from NYOB. But I would like him to stop conflating DBT and ABX - assuming that he is capable of understanding the difference. Ludovic Mirabel |
#8
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message oups.com... Arny Krueger wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Nice to hear from you, Arny. I've been looking for your contribution to the "The case for ABX..." thread. I thought you might be unwell or on leave but I see that you let your flock take the grapeshot. Well an ArchPriest has to save his irreplaceable self. Your answer to Middius allows two interpretations: 1) you do not understand that "posturing" refers to Nyob's appropriating a serious article 2) You're up to your debating trickery which I continue to think is not worthy of you. As for Ludovic needing lessons from fatuous NYOB in the value of double-blind testing{ For the umpteenth time:1) I was doing DBT medical research before any of you heard the name. Medical DBT research can be validated by objective data (psyche is an exception) 2) DBT is DBT, ABX is ABX 3) ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. I use blind technique sometimes myself when I listen to a new component. Not yours but mine Left-Right method.4) But when it comes to invalidable preferences in musical reproduction I'll take eg. Holt's opinion sighted any time over NYOBs nth power seventy times blind. Take whatever you want you still haven't a clue about ABX for audio. 5) Research is another matter entirely: double blind provides uniformity. I need no convincing from NYOB. You mean you've finally accepted that ABX is a valid protocol used to test for subtle audio differences? But I would like him to stop conflating DBT and ABX - assuming that he is capable of understanding the difference. Ludovic Mirabel I know that ABX is a form of DBT, something you don't seem to get. It is also a widely used and valid protocol, something else you don't seem to get. |
#9
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "dave weil" wrote in message ... On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 00:53:45 GMT, wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. I've got a modern newsreader that has the number of lines listed right next to the subject in the message list pane (and George has that same newreader). You should try such a newsreader sometime. Why? I don't care about how many lines in a post. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. |
#10
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "Arny Krueger" wrote in message ... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Odd since he's the one doing so much of the posturing. |
#11
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 07:58:02 GMT, wrote:
"dave weil" wrote in message .. . On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 00:53:45 GMT, wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. I've got a modern newsreader that has the number of lines listed right next to the subject in the message list pane (and George has that same newreader). You should try such a newsreader sometime. Why? I don't care about how many lines in a post. It would stop you from making stupid statements about "wasting time counting the number of lines". It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. I guess having your ignorance exposed again makes you sleepy. Cool. |
#12
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() The Krooborg puffs up its scrawny chest. 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing Speaking of showing, Turdy, how about you post that picture of your "system" again so the newest generation of RAO spectators can establish an accurate frame of reference for your vast "audio knowedge"? .. .. |
#13
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... Since we all know you are in desparate need of more information on how and why DBT's are used by Sean Olive and Floyd Toole I include the following for your edification. They make crap. It has been proven that ABX is used by these companies to determine the poorest quality that the public will accept. ABX has been proven not to work in hifi applications. |
#14
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. If you don't like what I write, don't read it. You are wasting usenet bandwidth. You are a bad usenet netizen. |
#15
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. Mikey, you are being repetitious. We covered this last month. We all know what the other side thinks. |
#16
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message ink.net... "dave weil" wrote in message ... On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 00:53:45 GMT, wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. I've got a modern newsreader that has the number of lines listed right next to the subject in the message list pane (and George has that same newreader). You should try such a newsreader sometime. Why? I don't care about how many lines in a post. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. Don't do this Mikey. You are a bad person, as well as stupid. |
#17
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message ink.net... wrote in message oups.com... Arny Krueger wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Nice to hear from you, Arny. I've been looking for your contribution to the "The case for ABX..." thread. I thought you might be unwell or on leave but I see that you let your flock take the grapeshot. Well an ArchPriest has to save his irreplaceable self. Your answer to Middius allows two interpretations: 1) you do not understand that "posturing" refers to Nyob's appropriating a serious article 2) You're up to your debating trickery which I continue to think is not worthy of you. As for Ludovic needing lessons from fatuous NYOB in the value of double-blind testing{ For the umpteenth time:1) I was doing DBT medical research before any of you heard the name. Medical DBT research can be validated by objective data (psyche is an exception) 2) DBT is DBT, ABX is ABX 3) ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. I use blind technique sometimes myself when I listen to a new component. Not yours but mine Left-Right method.4) But when it comes to invalidable preferences in musical reproduction I'll take eg. Holt's opinion sighted any time over NYOBs nth power seventy times blind. Take whatever you want you still haven't a clue about ABX for audio. 5) Research is another matter entirely: double blind provides uniformity. I need no convincing from NYOB. You mean you've finally accepted that ABX is a valid protocol used to test for subtle audio differences? But I would like him to stop conflating DBT and ABX - assuming that he is capable of understanding the difference. Ludovic Mirabel I know that ABX is a form of DBT, something you don't seem to get. It is also a widely used and valid protocol, something else you don't seem to get. We know what it is, and what it is used for. Not for hifi. |
#18
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "Arny Krueger" wrote in message ... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Odd since he's the one doing so much of the posturing. We know who Harmon is, and what they make, which is barely acceptable equipment. Ludovic has shown you that ABX is an invalid protocol for hifi. |
#19
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() Robert Morein said: 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. Don't do this Mikey. You are a bad person, as well as stupid. Mickey is following his and Arnii's religious rituals. Stupidity is one of their chief articles of faith. It ranks right up there with hypocrisy. A primo example of both the stupidity and hypocrisy of the 'borgs is their fanatical devotion to the depraved aBxism torture rituals. Their mindless, monotonous flogging of this intellectually bankrupt "testing" regimen is stupid because all the extant evidence shows it's worthless for consumers. In fact it's actually worse than worthless because any consumer who undertakes the blinding rituals not only doesn't learn anything useful, he also wastes his own time and/or money. The hypocrisy is even more salient: None of the 'borgs has EVER done an aBxism ritual before making a purchase for their own use. And yet they yammer on endlessly about how perfect the rituals are, how indispensable for "saving money". Hypocrisy of this intensity is worthy of the most two-faced politicians or potentates of organized religion. It reminds me of Jim Bakker and Tom Delay. .. .. .. |
#20
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "dave weil" wrote in message ... On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 07:58:02 GMT, wrote: "dave weil" wrote in message . .. On Mon, 14 Nov 2005 00:53:45 GMT, wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. I've got a modern newsreader that has the number of lines listed right next to the subject in the message list pane (and George has that same newreader). You should try such a newsreader sometime. Why? I don't care about how many lines in a post. It would stop you from making stupid statements about "wasting time counting the number of lines". It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. I guess having your ignorance exposed again makes you sleepy. Cool. Talking about how many lines in a post and the software that does it make me bored. No surprise you guessed wrong, again. |
#21
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "George Middius" wrote in message ... Robert Morein said: 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. Don't do this Mikey. You are a bad person, as well as stupid. Mickey is following his and Arnii's religious rituals. Stupidity is one of their chief articles of faith. It ranks right up there with hypocrisy. A primo example of both the stupidity and hypocrisy of the 'borgs is their fanatical devotion to the depraved aBxism torture rituals. Their mindless, monotonous flogging of this intellectually bankrupt "testing" regimen is stupid because all the extant evidence shows it's worthless for consumers. In fact it's actually worse than worthless because any consumer who undertakes the blinding rituals not only doesn't learn anything useful, he also wastes his own time and/or money. To that, Ludovic cites considerable evidence that ABX, as currently practiced, is useless for research in the "high end". He cites examples of amps known to be mediocre, which were indistinguishable in an ABX "test" from amps known to be superior. As chocolate dulls and confuses the palate, so does ABX dull and confuse the ear. |
#22
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "Arny Krueger" wrote in message ... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Krueger replies with #27, "Thanks for showing". It appears that Arny's entire phraseology fits neatly into five bits. The other three bits are presumably used by Krueger to define various Borg power-up states, which are strikingly similar to ACPI. |
#23
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. If you don't like what I write, don't read it. How would he know if he liked it, until after he read it? Oh, I get your message, just look at the specs such as line length, rather than actually read (and listen to) the contents. Its the Mikey way! |
#24
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? |
#25
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "George Middius" wrote in message ... Robert Morein said: 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. It took me about a nanosecond to see the number of lines in the post that Mr. Middius is referencing, although he rounded down. Yawn. Don't do this Mikey. You are a bad person, as well as stupid. Mickey is following his and Arnii's religious rituals. Stupidity is one of their chief articles of faith. It ranks right up there with hypocrisy. A primo example of both the stupidity and hypocrisy of the 'borgs is their fanatical devotion to the depraved aBxism torture rituals. Their mindless, monotonous flogging of this intellectually bankrupt "testing" regimen is stupid because all the extant evidence shows it's worthless for consumers. In fact it's actually worse than worthless because any consumer who undertakes the blinding rituals not only doesn't learn anything useful, he also wastes his own time and/or money. The hypocrisy is even more salient: None of the 'borgs has EVER done an aBxism ritual before making a purchase for their own use. And yet they yammer on endlessly about how perfect the rituals are, how indispensable for "saving money". Hypocrisy of this intensity is worthy of the most two-faced politicians or potentates of organized religion. It reminds me of Jim Bakker and Tom Delay. and John Kerry. |
#26
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "George Middius" wrote in message ... The Krooborg puffs up its scrawny chest. 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing Speaking of showing, Turdy, how about you post that picture of your "system" again so the newest generation of RAO spectators can establish an accurate frame of reference for your vast "audio knowedge"? he can't. His wife threw it out in last years spring cleaning |
#27
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? |
#28
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. |
#29
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#30
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message oups.com... wrote: wrote in message oups.com... Arny Krueger wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Nice to hear from you, Arny. I've been looking for your contribution to the "The case for ABX..." thread. I thought you might be unwell or on leave but I see that you let your flock take the grapeshot. Well an ArchPriest has to save his irreplaceable self. Your answer to Middius allows two interpretations: 1) you do not understand that "posturing" refers to Nyob's appropriating a serious article 2) You're up to your debating trickery which I continue to think is not worthy of you. As for Ludovic needing lessons from fatuous NYOB in the value of double-blind testing{ For the umpteenth time:1) I was doing DBT medical research before any of you heard the name. Medical DBT research can be validated by objective data (psyche is an exception) 2) DBT is DBT, ABX is ABX 3) ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. I use blind technique sometimes myself when I listen to a new component. Not yours but mine Left-Right method.4) But when it comes to invalidable preferences in musical reproduction I'll take eg. Holt's opinion sighted any time over NYOBs nth power seventy times blind. Take whatever you want you still haven't a clue about ABX for audio. 5) Research is another matter entirely: double blind provides uniformity. I need no convincing from NYOB. You mean you've finally accepted that ABX is a valid protocol used to test for subtle audio differences? But I would like him to stop conflating DBT and ABX - assuming that he is capable of understanding the difference. Ludovic Mirabel I know that ABX is a form of DBT, something you don't seem to get. It is also a widely used and valid protocol, something else you don't seem to get. __________________________________________________ _________ ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. BBC-- no. Quote documentation and show me for a liar or you for a ...what? Audio Info Audio Information Audio Info | Search | Statistics | Contact Us You are logged in as a guest. ( logon | register ) A bit from the BBC (Long) Audio Info- Audio Opinion Michael McKelvy Posted Jan 27, 2005 21:53:45 Subject: A bit from the BBC (Long) Guest The selection of loudspeakers for BBC Radio & Music. R. Walker, BBC Research and Development Department. 1 Introduction. The monitoring loudspeaker is unlike any other part of an audio production chain. It is the means by which the audio signal is judged. Other parts of the production chain certainly have effects, but they can be judged, altered or adjusted based on the final sound as reproduced by the loudspeakers. Historically, the BBC satisfied its requirements for studio monitoring loudspeakers by designing and developing its own range. The requirements could not generally be met at that time by contemporary commercial products, though some were used in special cases. Apart from the obvious need for high quality reproduction, the large number of studios and control rooms (more than 600 in the period around 1980) meant that the quality and character of the audio reproduction had to be maintained over a large number of units, over a range of loudspeaker sizes and for periods of many years. One of the main reasons for requiring consistency was (and still is) production practices that require operational staff to move between rooms for different jobs and for programme audio to be processed consecutively in different facilities (especially in television). Both of those factors require the sound monitoring in different rooms to be as similar as possible, otherwise much time and effort can be spent in doing and re-doing changes to the recorded material. That is unlike a commercial recording studio or small radio/TV station, where the technical staff work almost all the time in the same few rooms and can become familiar with their eccentricities. Those considerations led, over the years, to a succession of BBC-designed loudspeakers that also achieved substantial national and international recognition. The BBC-designed loudspeakers introduced several new developments, such as the use of polyethylene/ polypropylene (PE/PP) co-polymers for the diaphragm and Kapton? voice coil formers. The unit costs of BBC-designed loudspeakers were also significantly lower than equivalent commercial products, at that time. The last successful BBC-designed loudspeakers were the small LS3/5A (c. 1975), the large LS5/8 (c. 1982) and the medium LS5/9 (c. 1984). Those loudspeakers were the main ones in use from the dates of their original designs to the present. Very many are still in use at the present time (2004). In design, they may be well past their time and are, in any case, now irreplaceable. This author suggested in about 1992 that work should be started on their successors. At that time, the loudspeaker designs were between 8 and 17 years old. They are now between 20 and 29 years old. Three main factors led to the demise of the loudspeaker development work. With the coming of a more commercial imperative to the BBC as a whole, it was no longer considered worthwhile to develop loudspeakers internally. At that time (around 1994), the individual Directorates were made autonomous and responsible for managing themselves. That led to a lack of support for the sort of centralised activities needed to support loudspeaker development. Also, each business unit was isolated and indeed had been deliberately set up to be in internal competition with other units. The choice of loudspeakers was seen as an individual selection, based on each unit's own activities or cost considerations. That policy has led, over a period of about the last 10 years, to significant fragmentation, with many different types of loudspeakers being used in different areas. That has in turn led to the recognition (at least in some places) of the value of that earlier requirement for consistency. A second factor has been the availability of improved commercial loudspeakers. Over the period of about the last 40 years there is no doubt that commercial loudspeakers have improved dramatically. Even over the last 20 years since the design of the LS5/9, developments in commercial loudspeakers have led to improved properties, perhaps most of all in consistency. The third factor is the cost and complexity of modern loudspeakers. Years ago, a loudspeaker consisted of a few relatively simple components. Of course, the sound quality, reliability and other parameters depended on the choice of materials and their physical forms, but the principles were straightforward enough for a relatively small team to succeed in the design. Now, the additional refinements, the improvement in achievable quality and the complex design techniques make it less practicable for a small team to embark upon the development with any realistic hope of success. Designing high quality loudspeakers is an activity that can only be contemplated by a well-resourced team [1]. The addition of modern complex electronics, for room equalisation, for allowance for component variations or for the addition of digital control or input interfaces, further adds to the potential scale of the task. BBC Radio and Music (BBC R&M) considered that the scale of the project made it necessary to carry out a study to select a range of loudspeakers with the objective of setting up a Framework Agreement for the procurement of 'standard' loudspeakers. They invited News and World Service to take part, so that they could take advantage of the results if they wished. The size of the requirement made it necessary to go through a formal tendering process. It also exceeded the EU threshold, which meant that the EU Procedure (Restricted) process had to be applied. In order to include the quality of the loudspeaker as a selection parameter it was necessary to carry out formal listening tests. Even without that potential legal requirement, it was thought desirable to have a uniform loudspeaker installations throughout the new development. That required all potential user groups to support the selection process and to accept the outcome. It was also hoped that other Directorates might accept the idea of a new set of 'standard' BBC loudspeakers for future developments. Of course, under the existing separations between Directorates, there is no guarantee that will be so, but it would clearly be beneficial to the Corporation as a whole if it helped to keep down costs and contribute to uniformity of output. 2 The tendering process. In outline, the 'Restricted' EU tendering process consists of two stages. The first is an advertisement for potentially interested parties. The responses to the advertisement were studied and used to create a short list for the subsequent 'Invitations to Tender'. The Invitation called for proposals for three different sizes of loudspeaker to replace the LS5/8, LS5/9 and LS3/5A. It was made clear that the audio quality selection would be on the basis of subjective assessment alone[2]. However, a brief technical specification was included in the Invitation as guide to the general performance requirements. The subjective assessment was to be made on the basis first of absolute audio quality for each loudspeaker size and then on the 'family' resemblances between the three different sizes[3]. The set of three loudspeakers was considered as a family of products needed to fulfil the BBC requirements. They did not necessarily all have to be provided by a single manufacturer. The first level of assessment was the response to the initial advertisement. The accompanying questionnaire covered aspects of the supply of loudspeakers not directly related to their acoustic performance, such as company size and financial controls, staff health and welfare and company history. That was intended to eliminate commercially inappropriate proposals. At that stage, there was no exclusion of any potential supplier who might otherwise provide suitable loudspeakers. All respondents who met the commercial requirements were included, even though that resulted in more loudspeakers to be included in the assessments than had been thought practicable at the outset. Some of the proposals included the same loudspeakers in different combinations. It was clear that the final total of 19 different proposals was too many to assess. By removing the repetitions and the impractically expensive loudspeakers, the total number of proposals was reduced to 10. Each of the proposals included submissions for all three of the target sizes, so the total number of loudspeaker pairs actually to be assessed subjectively was nine large, ten medium and nine small (28 in total). 3 Test methodology. 3.1 The Parameters of an Ideal Test. A rigorous subjective test should consist of double blind, individual assessments using a suitable number of descriptive axes by a suitable number of test subjects. Probably five to ten subjective axes would be used. That is likely to be higher than the true number of different quality attributes, but would allow some scope for subsequent statistical factor reduction. Other tests doing similar things have commonly used around 20 descriptors. If the set of descriptors is too limited then test subjects feel constrained whilst having too many leads to confusion and overload of the test subjects. It was thought that not less than 10 test subjects would be needed, if they were highly skilled and could demonstrate a reasonable degree of consistency. For less able subjects, at least two or three times that number would have been necessary. The subjects were also being asked to assess many different aspects of loudspeaker quality at the same time. Less expert subjects would have found that difficult. Because of the background to the tests, it was thought that the test subjects should also be, in some sense, 'chosen' by their colleagues, so that the tests would be seen as reasonable and the final results more readily accepted by the majority of production staff who had not taken part directly. The total number of loudspeakers was also an issue. At the outset, it had been anticipated that as many as eight suppliers might provide samples of each of the three sizes, resulting in 24 loudspeakers (stereo pairs) to be assessed. In the event, the final number was 28 pairs. Some dummy or repeat assessments also had to be included. In particular, the first assessment each day had to be rejected because there was no way of indicating to the test subjects the likely range of quality. Subjective tests, at least absolute assessments, usually include 'anchor points' to guide the subjects in the range of responses to be expected. That was not feasible in this case because the loudspeakers had to be moved between presentations. It had also been decided at the outset that the existing range of BBC loudspeakers would not be included as references because that would unfairly bias the results towards the existing and familiar products. It was felt that the proposals should be assessed entirely on their own merits, not in comparison with existing products. With one dummy test added to each set, the final total number of quality assessments was 31. The time allowed for each person's assessment also has to be reasonable. It was thought unreasonable to ask subjects to begin immediately with completely unknown material so some time was allowed for familiarisation using the subject's own material. It was estimated that the familiarisation and assessment could not be done in less than 30 minutes. In loudspeaker listening, the acoustics of the room have a significant influence on the perceived sound. That, at least, would be the same for all loudspeakers. However, more significantly, the position of the loudspeaker in the room also has a profound effect. That meant the assessments had to be done with all of the loudspeakers in the same positions in the room and that each test had to include changing the physical positions of the loudspeakers. In addition, some people think that having another loudspeaker present anywhere in the room affects the perceived sound[4]. That meant not only did the loudspeakers have to be changed in position but actually removed from the room when not being assessed. All of that had to take place without the subject knowing what was happening. In practice, the test subjects would have to be moved to a separate waiting area while the loudspeakers were changed over and the new ones connected, tested and their levels set. All of that was thought likely to add at least 30 minutes to each assessment. So, it was thought that a full set of tests would consist of about 240 assessments. With 60 minutes each that would total about 240 hours of testing, or about 30 working days. There were two possibilities :- 1) One test could be carried out per day with one loudspeaker and all ten subjects. With the final number of loudspeakers, that would have taken a total of 28 days to test all of the loudspeakers. It would have meant all ten subjects attending the test venue on 28 separate occasions for one hour on each occasion. Taking into account the significant travelling time, even from central London never mind from the Regions, that would have been logistically impossible. 2) Alternatively, each of the test subjects could be present at the test venue for an extended period and the loudspeakers changed over. About eight tests per day might have been achieved. That would have made a total of about four days. The total time for the testing would therefore have been about 40 working days. However, it would have been utterly exhausting for the project team, who would have to arrange the loudspeakers in the room a total of 310 times and probably too exhausting for the subjects (who would have to concentrate very hard for most of the four days, admittedly with reasonable breaks every 30 minutes or so). Both of these ideal options were considered to be unworkable. Option '1' had the serious problem that no dummy or 'ranging' tests could be sensibly included. Each test subject would listen to only one loudspeaker each day and would have to carry mental recollections of the quality over a period of more than a month. That was clearly not sensible. Option '2' had the advantage of minimising the commitments for the test subjects, but involved an unacceptable amount of work for the project team. Both of the options would have taken too much time overall. It was clear that the procedure was going to have to be compromised in some way in order to make the tests practicable. 3.2 Possible Simplifications and Their Defects. A number of considerations applied to the simplification of the test procedu- 1. Whatever else happened, there had be some randomisation of the order of presentation to reduce the effects of order as a factor in the results. If that were not done, the results would depend very heavily on the presentation sequence. Even expert listeners would find it difficult to avoid their assessment being relative to the previous samples rather than independent of them. That would be impossible with scheme '1' but possible to do with a test programme based on scheme '2'. 2. More than one loudspeaker pair could be put in the room at the same time in as near to the optimum positions as possible (restricted significantly in some cases because of the size of some of the loudspeakers). That would simplify the changeover. However, that would unfairly influence the results because of room position effects, apart from any questions over interactions between the loudspeakers. 3. The obvious simplification was to allow several subjects to assess the loudspeakers at the same time. If that were done without allowing them to change seating positions then the results would again be heavily biased by the test subject's seat position. If they were allowed to move around during the session then there would inevitably be some risk of collusion. Collusion leads to effectively fewer test subjects, especially if there are one or two dominant personalities in the room. However, the test subjects were all experienced operators, with independent opinions. It was thought likely that they could form their own views, independent of their colleagues. (In the event, the results showed that most test subjects did form reasonably individual opinions.) That simplification would not have been acceptable with inexperienced test subjects. 4. Given the time-scale and scope of the testing, the only practicable test procedure would be to have all the sample loudspeakers on site and to invite all of the test subjects to be at the test venue for an intensive period of three or four days. 5. It was not possible to carry out the tests entirely 'double-blind'. Inevitably, the project team would know which loudspeakers were being assessed. However, only one member of the project team remained in the room to supervise the tests - to ensure that the test subjects did not confer or try to look behind the screen to identify the loudspeakers. That person was often actually unaware of the loudspeaker manufacturer because code letters were used to identify the loudspeakers. In any case, under the pressure of physically moving the loudspeakers and setting the levels, the members of the project team had little time (or inclination) to take note of the identity. The supervisor was also under strict instruction not to exhibit any reactions. 3.3 The Final Test Procedure - Absolute Quality. The final procedure for the quality tests consisted of one day's testing for each of the three sizes of loudspeaker. Each day consisted of 10 or 11 assessments (depending on the number of loudspeakers in the group). On occasions, errors in set up procedure or defects in equipment caused some assessments to be re-started. When the fault was clearly noticeable to the subjects and made it unrealistic to continue, for example one channel not working or a 'ringing' loudspeaker stand [5], the test was re-started immediately. When the fault was not directly evident, for example if a mistake had been made in the overall level setting, the whole test was repeated without the test subjects being aware and the first results discarded. The final programme allowed 45 minutes for each assessment, 30 minutes for the actual assessment and 15 minutes for the technical changeover. Longer breaks were included for refreshments and meals. Overall, the test programme was very demanding of both the project team and the subjects, with additional breaks having to be added sometimes. The test panel was nominally 12 subjects, but not all were present all of the time. Eleven test subjects attended all of the tests. Because it was thought that 12 at once were too many for the room, the test panel was split into two groups. The whole test procedure for quality assessment was therefore carried out twice, once for each group. The two test sessions were held in mid-December 2003 and early January 2004. The orders of presentation were different for the two groups. The reproduction level was controlled to within ?0.5 dB using a pre-recorded pink noise track on the test disk and a Bruel and Kj?r 2231 Precision Sound Level meter set to 'flat', located at the central front seat position. The pink noise recording was band-limited to 200 Hz - 8 kHz, removing the lowest frequencies to provide fair comparison signals for the smaller loudspeakers. The individual tracks of the test disk had been set subjectively to appropriate levels for the type of material by members of the project team. During the tests it was not necessary to adjust the levels. The test disk was simply allowed to play from beginning to end, with 13 tracks of about 1 minute each. A period of about 10 minutes was allowed before the formal test for the test subjects to use their own material for familiarisation. The quality assessments were carried out using two-channel stereophonic material, though some tracks were presented in left or right channels only. 3.4 The Test Material. The test material was selected to cover a range of genres. It included male and female speech, drama and several musical items. Not all of the material was the highest quality. Monitoring loudspeakers have to be able to present defective material accurately in order for the defects to be identified. Each extract lasted for about 1 minute. Thirteen tracks were finally selected for presentation. The recording medium was audio CD. Replay was by Studer A730 CD player (digital output) with a Prism 'Dream DA-1' digital-to analogue decoder. Most of the loudspeakers included built-in amplifiers. For the remainder, analogue power amplifiers were supplied by the system proposers. All of the systems tested used analogue audio inputs to the loudspeakers/amplifiers. 3.5 Absolute Quality Assement Parameters. The absolute quality was assessed on a total of 19 individual parameters divided into five main attributes and an overall score. The parameters were :- SPECTRAL UNIFORMITY - uncoloured tonal reproduction of the audio material, evenness of frequency response, granularity of frequency response, adequate, but non-emphasised low frequency response, adequate, but non-emphasised high frequency response. SOUND-STAGE IMAGING - Correctly located sound stage, continuous, even and wide left-right sound stage, localisation and separation of individual sound images, stable and open sound stage. AMBIENCE REPRODUCTION - spacious and diffuse reverberant sound, rendition of direct-to-reverberant sound ratio, rendition of recording sound space, uncoloured reproduction of reverberation. DYNAMICS AND DISTORTION - consistency of performance at reduced level, adequate rendition of material recorded at low level, level, purity and impact of loud sounds, lack of range compression and frequency intermodulation. OFF-AXIS PERFORMANCE - performance off the centre line, loudspeaker plane or with head movements. SUMMARY - Overall Impression. The 19 parameters were to be given a numerical score between 1 and 5, with '1' representing 'poor' and '5' representing 'good'. A space was left at the end for free-form comments. 3.6 Test Procedure - Family Resemblances. The second stage of the tests, assessing family resemblances was thought to be technically less problematical. It was arranged as a follow-on test, after the loudspeakers had been put in order of preference by the quality assessments. It was inescapable that all three of the loudspeakers being assessed for similarity should be in the room at the same time, to allow rapid switching between them. It was considered quite impossible for test subjects to assess or remember differences or similarities over longer periods of time. The absolute quality had already been decided, so that the effects of the room and the other loudspeakers on quality were less important. Because it was considered less critically dependent on the test conditions, all 11 of the test subjects were present at the same time. The three loudspeakers being compared were placed close together, so that room position effects would be minimised. The tests were carried out with monophonic signals, the source being the same test recordings with the two channels bridged together. The scoring sheet for the assessment of family resemblance consisted simply of three boxes for the two-way comparisons between the large, medium and small loudspeakers. The points were awarded as +2 for "very good match", 1 for "fairly good match", -1 for "slightly different" and -2 for "very different". The instructions were to assess and score each of the three comparisons. There was no fixed time limit; the test subjects were allowed to continue until all were satisfied that they had produced a fair assessment. The selection of groups for the family resemblance tests was guided by two main factors. Firstly, the best individual loudspeakers should be selected. Secondly, if the first consideration did not lead to an optimum selection for family resemblance then loudspeakers not scoring the highest in absolute quality might have to be included. A third and minor consideration, to be used to resolve otherwise closely-matched selections, was that preference should be given to loudspeakers from the same manufacturer. The family matching was considered to be a high priority. It was not so high that the absolute quality should be seriously compromised but high enough to allow for the rejection of the best individual loudspeakers. This was expected to be a difficult compromise to achieve, and so it proved to be. The technique adopted was first to try a few obvious groupings and then discuss with the test subjects other possible groupings. The test subjects remained unaware of the identity of the loudspeakers throughout. All discussion was carried out using the coded identifications. 4 Room acoustics and the test room. The room to be used for the tests was the subject of much discussion during the preparations for the tests. In the past, there had been a reasonably clear view of what a typical BBC control room was like, acoustically. Usually, they were (and many still are) heavily treated to produce much shorter reverberation times than most audience environments and to control early reflections by absorption. Many people have felt for some time that there should be a change in that basic room design. Indeed this author, in around 1991, began a process for change in the development of room acoustics that allowed much more 'lively' rooms, whilst still maintaining control of early reflections1,2,3,4,5. The new studio developments, and in particular, the development for which this loudspeaker selection process was being carried out, were (allegedly) to include much less acoustic treatment. It was thought best if a room more like the proposed new rooms could be used for the loudspeaker selection. In the event, that proved to be impossible. Firstly, it was difficult to find a room of any reasonable sort that was free for the necessary period. The use of reference listening rooms at BBC R&D had been ruled out on the grounds that they were too unrepresentative. Secondly, the messages coming from the design teams for new buildings suggested that the higher quality rooms at least would not be quite as different to present rooms as at first thought. That was more or less inevitable. Critical assessment of sound quality cannot be carried out in rooms with large areas of specularly-reflecting surfaces and long and uneven reverberations times, however attractive they might be architecturally. A room was eventually found that was available, centrally located and that had adjacent storage areas for the test loudspeakers[6]. The room had been a control cubicle for radio drama and so had the 'standard' BBC acoustic treatment already installed, though it had been disused for some time and was visually unattractive. To modify the room acoustically, it was made acoustically symmetrical by the addition of similar acoustic treatment over the existing observation window and some of the wall treatment was removed where it would not result in discrete early reflections in the listening area. Some patches of high frequency absorption were added to areas where equipment had been removed and the resulting bare patches would have caused reflections. To hide the wall surfaces, to make the room somewhat more attractive and thus not cause too much distraction for the test subjects, the entire inner wall surface was lined with lightweight fabric drapes. The final mid-band reverberation time was about 0.2 s. To provide for blind testing, an 'acoustically transparent' screen was erected across one end of the room, as an enclosure for the loudspeakers and amplifiers under test. The screen was made of lightweight black net fabric and lit by downlights so that the test subject could not see what was behind. The acoustic transmission properties of several different samples of material were measured and the one showing the least acoustic effect was selected. It showed a uniform progressive fall in transmitted sound level with increasing frequency, amounting to about 1 dB at 20 kHz. That would probably have been detectable in a direct comparison but it was the same for all of the loudspeakers tested and was considered to be acceptable. 5 Results. 5.1 Absolute Quality. The quality scores from the test subjects were averaged. That took into account the varying numbers of test subjects and their varying numbers of responses. It had not been insisted on that all subjects should score all attributes. Sometimes, subjects were unable to give numerical scores for all of the 19 attributes. Sometimes, they just gave an overall score for a group of attributes. In fact, there were only a very few instances of no score being given at all but there were perhaps 20% of cases where an aggregate score had been given for two or more attributes. The use of averages allowed the data processing to take account of those different numbers. The scores were averaged for the five quality attributes individually and overall. The overall scores were also calculated for the two groups of test subjects separately. The results from the quality assessments are shown in Figs. 1 to 4. Fig. 1 shows the overall average results for each of the loudspeakers for all three sizes. Figs. 2, 3 and 4 show the average results by attribute for each of the three sizes of loudspeaker. The 10 proposals were given code numbers 1 - 10 and the three sizes were given code letters 'a' for large, 'b' for medium and 'c' for small. In those cases where the same loudspeaker had been included in more than one proposal, the data was duplicated (e.g. a4/a8 and c4/c8). The loudspeakers themselves had not been assessed twice (as discussed above). Because the scale for scoring quality was from 1 to 5, an 'average' loudspeaker would score 3. There was a strong tendency for the test panel to avoid scoring either very high or very low. Individual scores of 1 were not given often, most test subjects probably thinking that they would reserve '1' for really poor loudspeakers, which did not in fact appear very often. At the other end of the range, scores of '5' were also comparatively rare, individuals probably reserving that for better loudspeakers than actual appeared. Certainly, there were many instances of loudspeakers being given scores of '1' (or even 0 in extreme cases) or '5' by individual test subjects who particularly disliked or liked a specific loudspeaker. However, those very polarised results tended to be swamped in the averaging, so that the overall results tended to be compressed into the middle range. An average of '2' would have actually indicated a poor quality loudspeaker and '4' an exceptionally good one [7]. Fig. 1 shows that, in fact, the entire range of average results for all three sizes was from 2.42 to 3.72. For the individual attributes, the total range of values was 2.08 to 4.0. That illustrates the compression of the results into a reduced range. In Fig. 1, some of the proposals scored either all poor (e.g. No. 7) or all good (e.g. Nos. 8, 9) over the range of sizes but in the majority of cases, the three sizes scored quite differently. That suggested that it might be difficult to select the best loudspeakers to make up family groups. In Figs. 2 to 4, the individual attributes were more consistent. Generally, those loudspeakers with poor overall results also scored poorly in all attributes, and vice versa. That was encouraging, as it showed loudspeakers could not be selected for particular attributes. Thereafter, only the overall scores were taken into account. The converse of that would have been very difficult to manage. If it really had been the case that some loudspeakers were especially suitable for some applications then selection of a 'standard' set of loudspeakers might have been impossible. 5.2 Family Resemblance. The Table 1 shows the results for the family resemblance tests, ordered by the overall family resemblance score. The total possible range for the score was -2 to +2, showing once again a compression of the actual results into a restricted range. Loudspeakers in test . . . Loudspeaker quality scores . . . Test # Overall average score Large Medium Small Large Medium Small Total-9 6 0.93 a5 b4 c5 3.55 3.21 3.61 1.37 9 0.67 a5 b5 c5 3.55 2.95 3.61 1.11 8 0.44 a2 b2 c5 3.49 3.35 3.61 1.45 3 0.23 a2 b2 c2 3.49 3.35 3.09 0.93 5 0.17 a9 b9 c9 3.28 3.34 3.42 1.04 7 0.13 a6 b4 c4 3.72 3.21 3.21 1.14 2 -0.30 a4 b4 c4 3.15 3.21 3.21 0.57 4 -0.87 a6 b3 c3 3.72 3.56 3.48 1.76 1 -1.13 a5 b3 c3 3.55 3.56 3.48 1.59 Table 1. Family resemblance results sorted by overall score. The columns are arranged as 'Number of the test' followed by the 'Score', followed by three columns listing the actual loudspeakers included in that test. Columns 6 to 8 give the overall average scores for those loudspeakers from the quality tests. The final column gives the total average quality score. In the final column, the differences have been emphasised by subtracting the average quality score for three loudspeakers of 9. The order of preference for family resemblance depended on which of the comparisons - large/medium, medium/small, large/small or overall - was used to order the list. The table shows the list ordered by the overall result. Other tables were generated (not shown) with ordering by the three individual two-way comparisons. The top four family test results for each of the four orders gave 6,9,3,8 / 6,5,9,8 / 8,6,9,5 and 6,9,8,3 as the preferences, for respectively the large/medium, medium/small, large/small comparisons. The shaded section in Table 1 indicates those best five candidates. The others scored too low on either the absolute quality or the family resemblance tests to be considered as contenders. Test 6 was obviously a good combination. It came top (three times) or second (once) in the family resemblance test results and had a reasonably high quality score, coming fourth overall. However, it did include a mix of manufacturers (the middle size was different). Test 9 produced lower results from the quality tests. It was thought to be too far down the quality preference order to be a first choice, even though the family resemblance positions were 2nd, 3rd, 3rd, and 2nd. Test 8 had very good results from the quality tests (3rd place overall and top out of the five best results for family resemblance). However, it produced lower results from the family resemblance tests, coming 4th, 4th, 1st and 3rd respectively. Test 3 only featured in two of the top four family resemblance results (sixth and seventh in the other two). It also had low results for quality. Test 5 produced poor result from quality tests, although all sizes got about the same score. It also featured in only two of the top four family resemblance test orders, coming 5th and 8th in the others. Of these results, Test 8 appeared to be a reasonable first choice. It achieved 3rd place overall in the quality tests and 3rd in the overall family resemblance results, but was only 4th in the important Large-Medium ordered list. Tests 6 and 9 were both though to be possibilities for a second choice. They differed only in the selection for the medium sized loudspeaker. They both came high in the results for family resemblance. Test 9 did have lower quality results but had the advantage of being a single manufacturer. 6 Final selection. The loudspeakers in Test #8 of the family resemblance tests were a2, b2 and c5, i.e. a mixed manufacturer group. The small loudspeaker from the same manufacturer as the two larger sizes, c2, only obtained an average score in the quality assessments and the family resemblance score was not high (Test #3). However, a mixed set was not considered unacceptable. The large and medium sizes, a2 and b2, were certainly very well liked. Taken together, they scored higher than any other pair of large/medium loudspeakers (Fig. 1). The small loudspeaker, c5, was the highest scoring one in its size category. Overall, that should have been a fairly clear winning combination, with perhaps a bias towards absolute quality. However, there were some serious reservations about the maximum sound level capabilities of the large loudspeaker. It was physically much smaller than others in the 'large' category. The original guide specification had indicated a maximum output level of 108 dB spl, which it was easily capable of at mid and high frequencies. However, even the manufacturer's own data showed very limited low frequency output - with measured indications of stress and distortion by 90/92 dB spl at 50/63 Hz. Listening tests confirmed that the limited low frequency output would be an operational restriction in many areas, though adequate for some others. One option would have been to select that loudspeaker as the standard for the large size and to specify something else in areas that required higher output. However, it was decided that would lead to too many special cases, maybe even the majority of 'large' loudspeaker installations, and would partially defeat the objective of selecting a standard set of loudspeakers. Therefore the selection was rejected. The next reasonable option was either Tests #6 or Test #9. They were the same except for the choice of the middle-sized loudspeaker. They both came high on the list of family resemblance results, but their results for absolute quality were substantially different. That led to an investigation of some very different results obtained for the 'b5' loudspeaker from the two groups of test subjects. The first group had given the loudspeaker an overall score of 3.84 and the second group 1.91. The first group thought the loudspeaker better than any other by a large margin. The second highest-placed medium loudspeaker by that group scored 3.50. The second group thought the loudspeaker worse than any other, though by a smaller margin. The next lowest placed medium loudspeaker by that group scored 2.17 [8]. That seemed to be a perverse anomaly that required further investigation. The discrepancy needed to be resolved, though the issue actually only involved the selection of the medium-sized loudspeaker, whether it was to be 'b4' or 'b5'. The two samples of loudspeaker 'b5' were listened to extensively by the project team in the test environment. It became clear that the two samples supplied were significantly different in sound quality. When placed together, the difference was striking. Of course, there was no indication of which one was more 'correct'. It was decided that further samples of the type should be obtained and assessed. An additional pair was acquired and assessed by the project team. That pair was more consistent and also matched one of the original pair fairly well. It was concluded that one of the original pair had been somewhat different. It was still not clear whether that represented normal manufacturing tolerances or a defective sample. The effect of the difference in the responses of the original pair might well have been to give a 'spread' in the stereo image quality, corresponding to a "pulling apart" or "stretching out" of the stereophonic images reported by some members of the project team. Such an effect might have been picked up by one group of test subjects but not by the other, especially if their attention was also being taken up by a long list of other factors. Further assessments, and some measurements, were carried out at BBC R&D Department, using another listening room and some other test subjects, comparing all four of the samples. The conclusion at the end of all of the supplementary tests was that the anomalous score given by the second group of test subjects was probably correct but that it had been based on a defective or atypical loudspeaker sample. The family of loudspeakers represented by 'a5', 'b5' and 'c5' had very closely matched manufacturing processes. Indeed, the project team were quite convinced that some of the actual drivers were the same in some cases, though the manufacturers did state that they were not precisely the same. In that context, it seemed unlikely that the loudspeakers should score significantly differently in the quality tests. In the end, it was recognised and accepted that the final choice for the medium-sized loudspeaker was based on somewhat uncertain evidence. The balance of probability, taking all the factors into account, was felt to be in favour of loudspeaker 'b5' as the preferred choice over 'b4'. It was also the case that loudspeaker 'b4' was already in use in significant numbers in some parts of Radio. Therefore, the existing loudspeakers could be re-deployed without conflicting with the requirement for family resemblance as it provided a good match and the existing stock would remain of some value. Thus, the final preferred selection for new purchases was 'a5' for the large sized loudspeaker, 'b5' for the medium sized loudspeaker and 'c5' for the small sized loudspeaker. 7 Summary and conclusions. This report has presented the background to the requirements for a range of new, standard loudspeakers for BBC Radio & Music. That required a set of three sizes, to replace the BBC designed LS5/8, LS5/9 and LS3/5A still in use in many areas. Because of the size of the potential order, there was in any case a requirement to pursue a formal tendering and selection process for the supply of the loudspeakers. A project was set up to carry out the tendering process and to organise formal subjective tests on potential candidates as part of the selection process. The selection was to be made not only on the absolute quality of the loudspeakers but also on their family resemblance so that a more uniform sound quality could be achieved in a range of applications. The tests involved operational studio managers from many areas of radio as test subjects, proposed by their colleagues as being expert and credible judges of sound quality. The intention was to try to ensure that all staff potentially affected would have contributed in some way to the process and would, as a result, more readily accept the outcome. Loudspeakers are a sensitive topic on which many people have very different views and it was anticipated that reservations could be expressed by some. By trying to involve as many people as possible, even only in a peripheral sense, it was hoped that the transition to new loudspeakers could be made as smooth and trouble-free as possible. Inevitably, the final outcome was not entirely clear-cut. It would have been simple if a single set of loudspeakers from one manufacturer had stood out from the rest in quality and family matching. In the event, the best loudspeakers in each size category were not selected because of the family matching requirements and other technical performance issues, but the final compromise was still close to the best. The final choice consisted of three different sizes of loudspeaker from the same manufacturer. That should have benefits for consistency in the future, especially if that manufacturer were to change the production method or introduce new models. It should be noted that many other factors were also included in the selection process. They included weight, size, maintainability and many others. The acoustic differences were taken into consideration amongst those other factors in the overall selection. 8 Acknowledgements. This work was managed and led by Stuart Collins of Radio Resources. Many other people from different departments contributed to the selection process. The author acknowledges the substantial contributions of all of those people, whose work this paper mainly represents. The author gratefully acknowledges the permission granted by the BBC for publication of this paper. 9 References. 1. Walker, R. A new approach to the design of control room acoustics for stereophony. AES Preprint No. 3543 (G1-1), 94th AES Convention, Berlin, March 16-19, 1993. 2. Walker, R. Early reflections in studio control rooms: the results from the first Controlled Image design installations. AES Preprint No. 3853 (P12-6), 96th AES Convention, Amsterdam, February 26-March 01, 1994. 3. Walker, R. Controlled Image Designs: the management of stereophonic image quality. BBC R&D Report No. 1995/4. 4. Walker, R. Controlled Image Designs: the measurement of time-frequency responses. BBC R&D Report No. 1995/3. 5. Walker, R. Controlled Image Designs: results from the first installations. BBC R&D Report No. 1995/5. RW 19/08/04 1] It is true that there are still some individual designers working in the field. Generally, they produce loudspeakers with individualistic characteristics, which may well be excellent in their field of application, but which do not satisfy the BBC's requirements for a widely used, standard loudspeaker. [2] Other non-audio factors, such as handling, maintainability, interchangeability, etc. were also important, as were business considerations not specifically related to loudspeakers, but they were not part of the audio quality selection process. [3] Throughout this selection process, the terms 'large', 'medium' and 'small' were used to identify the three broad categories of loudspeaker. In the design, it is inevitable that more compromises have to be made to obtain good performance from a smaller loudspeaker. The use of loudspeakers is frequently constrained by the space available and the accompanying reductions in performance have to be accepted. For cost-sensitive applications, smaller loudspeakers also usually cost less. [4] The author does not subscribe to this view. Most loudspeakers are so inefficient electrically and are in any case normally driven from a virtually zero impedance source that the possibility of received sound being re-radiated after being converted to electrical signals and back again is entirely negligible. The box itself is an object in the room, but no more significant than any other piece of furniture. Some loudspeakers have open resonant ports for low frequency tuning, but the damping/reverberation time of those resonators is entirely swamped by the room itself (in any reasonable room and with any reasonable loudspeakers). [5] The suppliers had been asked to provide stands if they thought it necessary to do so. Otherwise, ordinary metal frame stands were used. In more than one case, the manufacturer's own stand was found to be unacceptable. [6] Maida Vale, Studio 7 control cubicle. [7] All of the loudspeakers assessed in this trial were from the upper end of high quality professional monitoring loudspeakers and that a 'poor' score might still represent a loudspeaker that was good or excellent in terms of absolute performance for other applications. [8] It was also true that the second group gave lower scores overall. The overall average scores from the quality tests for the two groups were 3.34 and 2.97 respectively. Given the compression of scores into a relatively narrow range, that represented quite a marked difference. Michael McKelvy Posted Jan 28, 2005 17:18:21 Subject: A bit from the BBC (Long) Guest "Ruud Broens" |broensr([at]wanadoo.nl| said 41f98e49$0$54524$a344fe98([at]news.wanadoo.nl... | | "Michael McKelvy" |deskst49([at]peoplepc.com| said | JpdKd.836$Pi.315([at]newsread3.news.atl.earthlink.net... | : | : The selection of loudspeakers for BBC Radio & Music. | : | : | : R. Walker, BBC Research and Development Department. | .................................................. .............................. |
#31
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#32
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. And? |
#33
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() The Bug Eater allows us to expand the Mickey McMickey Codex. 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. And? Thanks Mr. McMickey for, divulging the correct pronunciation of "duh" in Bug-Eaterese. .. .. |
#34
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() "George Middius" wrote in message ... The Bug Eater allows us to expand the Mickey McMickey Codex. 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. And? Thanks Mr. McMickey for, divulging the correct pronunciation of "duh" in Bug-Eaterese. Oooooh, that hurts. You may now return to your turd sandwich. |
#36
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... 1831 lines and two useless "gif"ts |
#37
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. And? Your incompetence was showing. i just wanted to give you a chance to zip up your fly. |
#39
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() "Clyde Slick" wrote in message news ![]() wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "Clyde Slick" wrote in message ... wrote in message nk.net... "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message ... 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. What the **** are doing wasting time counting the number of lines, get a hobby already. 2,560 exactly, and I didn't ahve to count them. Having trouble reading usenet specs, Mikey?? Just don't care. Is there some reason I should? Yes indeed, you were mouthing off about them, without knowing anything about them. And? Your incompetence was showing. i just wanted to give you a chance to zip up your fly. I'm sorry, does this mean I can't join the usenet geeks club and compare how many lines in each post? |
#40
![]()
Posted to rec.audio.opinion
|
|||
|
|||
![]() wrote in message oups.com... wrote: I hope this e-mail address of yours works because I just sent some .pdf files that were sent to by Sean Olive that clearly indicate that B&O, NASA, and Nokia all use ABX. wrote in message oups.com... wrote: wrote in message oups.com... Arny Krueger wrote: "George M. Middius" cmndr [underscore] george [at] comcast [dot] net wrote in message 2500 lines of posturing, fer chrissakes. Shut up, Mikey. Thanks for showing once again that you can't tell the difference between audio knowlege and posturing, Middius. Nice to hear from you, Arny. I've been looking for your contribution to the "The case for ABX..." thread. I thought you might be unwell or on leave but I see that you let your flock take the grapeshot. Well an ArchPriest has to save his irreplaceable self. Your answer to Middius allows two interpretations: 1) you do not understand that "posturing" refers to Nyob's appropriating a serious article 2) You're up to your debating trickery which I continue to think is not worthy of you. As for Ludovic needing lessons from fatuous NYOB in the value of double-blind testing{ For the umpteenth time:1) I was doing DBT medical research before any of you heard the name. Medical DBT research can be validated by objective data (psyche is an exception) 2) DBT is DBT, ABX is ABX 3) ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. I use blind technique sometimes myself when I listen to a new component. Not yours but mine Left-Right method.4) But when it comes to invalidable preferences in musical reproduction I'll take eg. Holt's opinion sighted any time over NYOBs nth power seventy times blind. Take whatever you want you still haven't a clue about ABX for audio. 5) Research is another matter entirely: double blind provides uniformity. I need no convincing from NYOB. You mean you've finally accepted that ABX is a valid protocol used to test for subtle audio differences? But I would like him to stop conflating DBT and ABX - assuming that he is capable of understanding the difference. Ludovic Mirabel I know that ABX is a form of DBT, something you don't seem to get. It is also a widely used and valid protocol, something else you don't seem to get. __________________________________________________ _________ ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. BBC-- no. Quote documentation and show me for a liar or you for a ...what? B&O- no. Ditto, Ditto, Ditto Nokia that famous audio company uses it for comparing cellphones or what? Documantation? Harman -no. Ditto, Ditto, Ditto Have you got the distinction between DBT and ABX clear in your head yet? NYOB, NYOB, you make my day. Ludovic Mirabel __________________________________________________ _______ . This was the exchange on Nov 16th in this thread. There were two postings by NYOB The first one can be looked up in that thread just above this one. NYOB: ABX is a double blind protocol used by people doing audio research from the BBC to Nokia, to Bang&Olafson, to Harman, to hearing aid companies, and on and on. I answered: BBC-- no. Quote documentation and show me for a liar or you for a ...what? B&O- no. Ditto, Ditto, Ditto Nokia that famous audio company uses it for comparing cellphones or what? Documantation? Harman -no. Ditto, Ditto, Ditto Have you got the distinction between DBT and ABX clear in your head yet? NYOB, NYOB, you make my day. Ludovic Mirabel In response I got 2 postings . The first one can be read entire in the thread. Even with "hiddeb text" it would make this too long for one posting. Below is my response to the two. You are my one and only, inimitable, incredible, fatuous, transsubstantial NYOB. With a wave of your wand you transport me to the "Alice in Wonderland" country. Whuite Rabbits, Mad Hatters all over the place. So far you produced some 60 phony references to the good, old Humpty-Dumpty ABX. This is the latest one- 7 meters long.. Ther was nothing about ABX in most of those , except for a few of them with a negative, "no diference" component comparisons.There is not one word about ABX in this one. The closest it gets is an incomplete DBT. Quote.: " It was not possible to carry out the tests entirely 'double-blind. Inevitably, the project team would know which loudspeakers were being assessed. However, only one member of the project team remained in the room to supervise the tests - to ensure that the test subjects did not confer or try to look behind the screen to identify the loudspeakers. That person was often actually unaware of the loudspeaker manufacturer because code letters were used to identify the loudspeakers. In any case, under the pressure of physically moving the loudspeakers and setting the levels, the members of the project team had little time (or inclination) to take note of the identity. The supervisor was also under strict instruction not to exhibit any reactions." End of quote. In the 2nd posting wrote: I hope this e-mail address of yours works because I just sent some .pdf files that were sent to by Sean Olive that clearly indicate that B&O, NASA, and Nokia all use ABX. I got by email 3 Acrobat attachments about a research project. I didn't read it in detail but it seemed to have nothing to do with ABX-as usual. Thanks for admitting you don't try to find the pertinent references. Who cares what it "indicates" to NYOB? All we need is just one paragraph: "We used ABX as testing method with this result:..." All of those attachments had something to do with ABX. NYOB- I get enough unasked for garbage by email as it is. You're welcome to wish me a Merry Xmas but if you have a contribution to a forum discussion publish it in the forum. Having to cope with offers of Cialis, penis enlargement and young women pining to entertain me I get enough tempting spam as it is. Secondly: If you found something about ABX (or whatever interested you) publish the quote with a reference not the whole bloody paper to waste time on for no benefit. You won't have to worry, since you are completely dishonest on thesubject of ABX and DBT, there's no reaosn to talk to you about it anymore. Yo already know that it's used by nearly everyone sho does any kind of audio research, denying it doesn't change that. You could e-mail Sean Olive and find out far more than I can tell, you, but you won't If you had ever tried to publish a scientific paper you wouldn't get very far if you snowed the editor under with your complete copies to wade through Soon you'll be copying books in the forum. Instead of talking about papers I'm not going to write, you'd be better of reading some that have to with the subject you claim to have some interest in. It is difficult to fathom what motivates you. I like to show fools that they arn't fooling anybody. I doubt if you're sophisticated enough to be just an ordinary liar. I'm considering several hypotheses: You have problems with simple verbal comprehension- you fall asleep before you get to the end of it- you think I'll fall asleep- which is it? No further response to further pointless mailings. Go in the corner and mouth: "I'm not a liar. Ludovic is a liar if he says I'm a liar. Ludovic Mirabel We all know what you are Ludo. **** off. |