Home |
Search |
Today's Posts |
|
#1
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website
I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. If a single person can detect a change reliably only 0.5% more often than by chance alone, controlled testing (including possible ABX) can absolutely be used to confirm that ability to a high degree of certainty. Absolutely. HOWEVER, if I wish to demonstrate THE PROBABLE ABSENCE a small difference (say 0.22/0.17) to a level of significance of .05 I require a very large sample. Moreover, if 32 People all find that they can identify a given change to a ..2 certainty level the whole group requires to be aggregated and requires to now allow the audibility of the suggested effect. However, if I insist that ALL 32 People show a .05 certainty level I could claim (and thius what is happening quite frequently) that no-one could hear the effect with any significant certainty, while I have in fact data that supports the position that the effect is audible. For those who have not studied Statistics and wish to get a reasonable idea of the position and why I will continue to insist on it are invited to consult the discussion previously published in Stereophile: The Highs & Lows of Double-Blind Testing Do not be alarmed by the source in a subjectivist audio magazine, I feel the discussion presented there is completely fair and allows both sides to represent their positions well. I leave it to the genteele reader to draw their own conclusions. Here just one quote I absolutely love: "When data are nonsignificant, one scientist may conclude that differences are inaudible, another may conclude that it is wiser to withhold judgment (because, for example, it is always possible that ancillary equipment used in the listening test masked otherwise audible differences), another may decide to issue challenges, and a fourth scientist may decide to have spare ribs for dinner. These four scientists, having decided what interpretation to make when listening data are nonsignificant, may be interested in the probability that their significance test will label data as nonsignificant when differences are audible, forcing them to make that interpretation rather than correctly conclude that differences are audible. For example: ..Scientist 1 wants to know the risk of concluding that differences are inaudible when differences are, in fact, audible. ..Scientist 2 wants to know the risk that he will withhold judgment when differences are, in fact, audible. ..Scientist 3 wants to know the risk that he will issue challenges when differences are, in fact, audible. ..Scientist 4 is beneath contempt because he is eating spare ribs while I am hungry and writing this damn letter!" You may go there by using the following: http://www.diyaudio.com/forums/showt...1&goto=newpost |
#2
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Michael McKelvy" wrote in message
ink.net This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. If a single person can detect a change reliably only 0.5% more often than by chance alone, controlled testing (including possible ABX) can absolutely be used to confirm that ability to a high degree of certainty. Absolutely. HOWEVER, if I wish to demonstrate THE PROBABLE ABSENCE a small difference (say 0.22/0.17) to a level of significance of .05 I require a very large sample. I really don't care about what people can't hear, I care about what people can hear and the experiences they go through in order to do that. As far as what people don't hear, the most important and meaningful results are obtained when people try to hear these vanishingly small differences for themselves. That's why the www.pcabx.com web site exists - for people to try to hear small differences for themselves. For example, as you may know, several of us have reliably heard small diffrences between good power amps. Knowing what we went through to accomplish that, and what we actually heard in terms of an audible difference, is the most personally important finding. Would I even spit in a bucket over the difference I heard? No way! |
#3
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Arny:
At the time the Stereophile article was written or at any point since, has Dave Clark had any financial in ABX devices? |
#4
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Michael McKelvy" wrote in message
ink.net At the time the Stereophile article was written or at any point since, has Dave Clark had any financial (interest) in ABX devices? Good question. I don't remember if we had sold ABX by 1985 or not. That's when his financial interest in ABX ended. |
#5
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Michael McKelvy" wrote in message
link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile |
#6
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"John Atkinson" wrote in message
om "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . Except this isn't an artible about double-blind testing. It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing. |
#7
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Arny Krueger" wrote in message
"John Atkinson" wrote in message om "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . Since the normals who support Atkinson love to make a big point out of trivail spelling errors: Correction: Except this isn't an article about double-blind testing. It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing. |
#8
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Sat, 4 Sep 2004 12:50:14 -0400, "Arny Krueger"
wrote: Since the normals who support Atkinson love to make a big point out of trivail spelling errors: As do you, of course. |
#9
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
dave weil said:
Since the normals who support Atkinson love to make a big point out of trivail spelling errors: As do you, of course. According to Arny-logic, he would be an Atkinson supporter as well as a "normal" (note quotation marks as to not confuse Arny any further). Who said a negative can't be proven? ;-) -- Sander deWaal "SOA of a KT88? Sufficient." |
#10
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article ,
dave weil wrote: On Sat, 4 Sep 2004 12:50:14 -0400, "Arny Krueger" wrote: Since the normals who support Atkinson love to make a big point out of trivail spelling errors: As do you, of course. And provides further evidence for the proposition that any commentary on spelling will itself contain a typo. Stephen |
#11
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Arny Krueger" wrote in message
... "John Atkinson" wrote in message om You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . Except this isn't an artible about double-blind testing. It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing. It looks as if you didn't read it, Mr. Krueger. The meat of the reprint is an article by Les Leventhal, of the University of Manitoba's Psychology Department, based on his Audio Engineering Society paper, "How Conventional Statistical Analyses Can Prevent Finding Audible Differences In Listening Tests," Preprint 2275 (C-9), which had been presented at the 79th AES Convention in New York, October 1985 and later reprinted in the JAES, meaning it had passed peer review. In addition to Mr. Leventhal's contributions, the reprint included comments from myself and from J. Gordon Holt, as well as from Tom Nousaine, David Carlstrom, David Clark, and E. Brad Meyer. Are you seriously suggesting that none of us have a clue about the "basics of subjective testing"? John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile |
#12
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"John Atkinson" wrote in message
om "Arny Krueger" wrote in message ... "John Atkinson" wrote in message om You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 .. Note that Atkinson quickly becomes totally confused and transfers my comment on a Stereophile article to an AES paper Except this isn't an artible about double-blind testing. It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing. It looks as if you didn't read it, Mr. Krueger. It looks to me like you are free-associating, Mr. Atkinson. The meat of the reprint is an article by Les Leventhal, of the University of Manitoba's Psychology Department, It's true that your article contains very little meat Atkinson, and what little meat it does contain was not written by you. based on his Audio Engineering Society paper, "How Conventional Statistical Analyses Can Prevent Finding Audible Differences In Listening Tests," Preprint 2275 (C-9), which had been presented at the 79th AES Convention in New York, October 1985 and later reprinted in the JAES, meaning it had passed peer review. My comments also seem apply to Levinthal's paper, as the subsequent disembowelments of it in the JAES (cited below) show quite clearly. Leventhal's little rant passed peer review just long enough for subsequent disembowelment. BTW Atkinson, you left out the two published corrections/partial retractions by Levinthal. Then there are the subsequent JAES papers that disemboweled Leventhal's papers: Comments on "Type 1 and Type 2 Errors in the Statistical Analysis of Listening Tests" and Author's Replies 674942 bytes (CD aes4) Author(s): Shanefield, Daniel; Clark, David; Nousaine, Tom; Leventhal, Les Publication: Volume 35 Number 7/8 pp. 567·572; July 1987 Transformed Binomial Confidence Limits for Listening Tests 468821 bytes (CD aes5) Author(s): Burstein, Herman Publication: Volume 37 Number 5 pp. 363·367; May 1989 Abstract: A simple transformation of classical binomial confidence limits provides exact confidence limits for the results of a listening test, such as the popular ABX test. These limits are for the proportion of known correct responses, as distinguished from guessed correct responses. Similarly, a point estimate is obtained for the proportion of known correct responses. The transformed binomial limits differ, often markedly, from those obtained by the Bayesian method. Approximation Formulas for Error Risk and Sample Size in ABX Testing 442116 bytes (CD aes4) Author(s): Burstein, Herman Publication: Volume 36 Number 11 pp. 879·883; November 1988 Abstract: When sampling from a dichotomous population with an assumed proportion p of events having a defined characteristic, the binomial distribution is the appropriate statistical model for accurately determining: type 1 error risk (symbol); type 2 error risk (symbol); sample size n based on specified (symbol) and (symbol) and assumptions about p; and critical c (minimum number of events to satisfy a specified [symbol]). Table 3 in [1] pre;sents such data for a limited number of sample sizes and p values. To extend the scope of Table 3 to most n and p, we present approximation formulas of substantial accuracy, based on the normal distribution as an approximation of the binomial. In addition to Mr. Leventhal's contributions, the reprint included comments from myself and from J. Gordon Holt, as well as from Tom Nousaine, David Carlstrom, David Clark, and E. Brad Meyer. Inability to distinguish between the Stereophile article I mentioned, and an AES paper I didn't mention noted. Are you seriously suggesting that none of us have a clue about the "basics of subjective testing"? Since "the none of us"were from an AES paper I wasn't commenting on, I'm not commenting on them. Are you totally confused or what, Atkinson? Why are you so hot to cite a paper that was subsequently debunked so thoroughly? |
#13
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Arny Krueger" wrote in message
... "John Atkinson" wrote in message om "Arny Krueger" wrote in message ... "John Atkinson" wrote in message om You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 Note that Atkinson quickly becomes totally confused and transfers my comment on a Stereophile article to an AES paper As I said, Mr. Krueger, you didn't appear to read the article at the link I gave, which includes a summarized version of the text of Professor Leventhal's AES paper. That's why I pointed this out to you. Except this isn't an artible about double-blind testing. It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing. It looks as if you didn't read it, Mr. Krueger... snip of Mr. Kruger's opinoions of the Leventhal AES paper, which I am sure he believes to be true but are irrelevant to the specific point I was making In addition to Mr. Leventhal's contributions, the reprint included comments from myself and from J. Gordon Holt, as well as from Tom Nousaine, David Carlstrom, David Clark, and E. Brad Meyer. Inability to distinguish between the Stereophile article I mentioned, and an AES paper I didn't mention noted. As I pointed out, the article and the AES paper are essentially the same. However, the Stsreophile article includes comments form several authors, not just Leventhal, which you would have known had you read past the first page. :-) Are you seriously suggesting that none of us have a clue about the "basics of subjective testing"? Since "the none of us" were from an AES paper I wasn't commenting on, I'm not commenting on them. The authors mentioned contributed to the Stereophile article to which you were referring, not the AES paper that was also summarized in the article. So it is fair to assume that your comment "It's really true confessions and misapprehensions of people who are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing" refers to me, to Gordon Holt, to Les Leventhal, and to Tom Nousaine, David Carlstrom, David Clark, and E. Brad Meyer, all of whom contribued to the Stereophile reprint to which you were referring. As I said, had you actually read past the first page of the article at http://www.stereophile.com/features/141, you would have known this. As it now stands, you are on record as stating that your associates Tom Nousaine, David Clark, and David Carlstrom "are pretty clueless about even the basics of subjective testing." :-) I am sure they will explain to you that you are wrong about this. :-) John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile |
#14
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article , "Arny Krueger"
wrote: Leventhal's little rant passed peer review just long enough for subsequent disembowelment. Arny, are you really stupid enough to belive what you claim? Then there are the subsequent JAES papers that disemboweled Leventhal's papers: Comments on "Type 1 and Type 2 Errors in the Statistical Analysis of Listening Tests" and Author's Replies 674942 bytes (CD aes4) Author(s): Shanefield, Daniel; Clark, David; Nousaine, Tom; Leventhal, Les Publication: Volume 35 Number 7/8 pp. 567·572; July 1987 This is a letter to the editor, and not a regular journal article. See below for detailed comments on this, after we check the only real journal articles you cited. Transformed Binomial Confidence Limits for Listening Tests 468821 bytes (CD aes5) Author(s): Burstein, Herman Publication: Volume 37 Number 5 pp. 363·367; May 1989 Abstract: A simple transformation of classical binomial confidence limits provides exact confidence limits for the results of a listening test, such as the popular ABX test. These limits are for the proportion of known correct responses, as distinguished from guessed correct responses. Similarly, a point estimate is obtained for the proportion of known correct responses. The transformed binomial limits differ, often markedly, from those obtained by the Bayesian method. There is nothing in this article that debunks Leventhal's 1986 JAES article "Type 1 and Type 2 Errors in the Statistical Analysis of Listening Tests" which is the final version of the preprint. The only places that Burstein even mentions Leventhal's JAES article are in footnotes: "Leventhal in [3] also justly suggests that c/n be evaluated in terms of the probability of type 2 error." and "The relationship between p_k and p_c in Eq. (2) also applies to other instances where it is desired to differentiate between the known response rate and the correct response rate. For example, Leventhal [3] in his Table 3 lists several effect sizes, namely, hypothesized correct-response rates in the population. These can be converted to known-response rates by Eq. (2). Thus an effect size of 0.75 becomes a known-response rate of (2 X 0.75) - 1 = 0.50. It is assumed here, as in Leventhal's Table 3, that a subject is choosing between two components. If more than two components are involved, it would be necessary to use Eq. (2a) below." Other than a bibliography entry, that is it. Burstein does mention a different Leventhal paper (in the BAS Speaker) and he indicates that he prefers p_k to p_c, but these are equivalent ways to express effect size. There is no debunking of Leventhal in this paper. Arny, you claimed this disemboweled Leventhal's paper, but you can't find anything in this paper to support your claim. Put up or shut up. Approximation Formulas for Error Risk and Sample Size in ABX Testing 442116 bytes (CD aes4) Author(s): Burstein, Herman Publication: Volume 36 Number 11 pp. 879·883; November 1988 Abstract: When sampling from a dichotomous population with an assumed proportion p of events having a defined characteristic, the binomial distribution is the appropriate statistical model for accurately determining: type 1 error risk (symbol); type 2 error risk (symbol); sample size n based on specified (symbol) and (symbol) and assumptions about p; and critical c (minimum number of events to satisfy a specified [symbol]). Table 3 in [1] pre;sents such data for a limited number of sample sizes and p values. To extend the scope of Table 3 to most n and p, we present approximation formulas of substantial accuracy, based on the normal distribution as an approximation of the binomial. Far from debunking Leventhal's paper, this is a whole-hearted endorsement of it. It starts as follows. "This paper is principally an extension of Table 3 in the paper by Leventhal [1], which correctly stresses the frequent importance of considering type 2 error risk as well as type 1 in testing a sample from a dischotomous population." Burstein goes on to write: "Our purpose is to provide approximation formulas that extend the scope of Leventhal's Table 3 to most values of sample size n and proportion p." Later, as he presents his formulas, he gives worked examples and shows that their numerical values agree with Leventhal's. Arny, you claimed this disemboweled Leventhal's paper, but you can't find anything in this paper to support your claim. Put up or shut up. There remains the "letter to the editor" that you cited. Shanefield observes that Leventhal provides some mathematical precision, and he cautions that statistical and practical significance are not the same. In short, there is no serious conflict with Leventhal's paper. Leventhal, of course does not disembowel his own paper. As another poster observed, Nousaine is confused about probability. (Were he correct in his view, he'd think that when a subject is guessing with p = .50, only half the trials were real trials!) Clark's letter is a rant, but it mostly shows that Clark doesn't seem to understand elementary statistics at all, but that is not new; it has been the case as far back as his 1982 JAES paper introducing ABX. Also, in this letter to the editor, Clark says he never claimed that differences were inaudible. Perhaps he forgot to read his own JAES paper, where he wrote "A 12-bit companded digital delay line was just audible. A 16-bit linear system was not." By the way, when I went back to Clark's original JAES paper, I noticed several problems. The overall impression is that Clark simply didn't really understand how using statistical methods works in science, but he knew enough to look scientific to an untrained reader. Clark appears to make the common mistake about what the p-value computed in a standard statistical test is. (It is NOT the "probability that a particular score from an A/B/X test is due to chance".) When Clark mentions a JAES paper by Plenge et al as an example of how Clark can use statistical methods to increase sensitivity, he commits a gaffe comparable to using an AC voltmeter to measure the (DC) voltage of a battery. Plenge's experiment involved Same/Different trials where each subject was presented with 10 Same and 10 Different pairs of filters. Since the design of that experiment fixed the numbers of same and different trials, it was not inherently appropriate to apply a binomial distribution. Clark's conclusion that some filters were audible is based on Clark's bogus misapplication of the binomial distribution formula; Plenge does not do that. Ironically, the design of ABX cleverly avoids that particular problem. Statistical science is more than simple calculations---it requires understanding of how and when to use the techniques. Clark's rant about Leventhal's JAES paper has to be seen as the work of someone who simply does not grasp the issues involved. Are you totally confused or what, Atkinson? Why are you so hot to cite a paper that was subsequently debunked so thoroughly? Arny, why are you so hot to cite references that you either haven't read, or don't understand? The only debunking here is of your claims. |
#15
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() "John Atkinson" wrote in message om... "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile John, Good article. I've subscribed now for a year, and IMHO, the magazine has lost it's spice. It's still a good magazine, but, IMHO, a bit too much "The Village of the Happy Nice People." Magnets for controversy do better in the long term. |
#16
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Robert Morein" wrote in message
"John Atkinson" wrote in message om... "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile John, Good article. It's a biased misleading POS designed to appeal to people who want to believe in magic, not science. |
#17
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Sun, 5 Sep 2004 05:08:52 -0400, "Arny Krueger"
wrote: "Robert Morein" wrote in message "John Atkinson" wrote in message om... "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile John, Good article. It's a biased misleading POS designed to appeal to people who want to believe in magic, not science. From the article: The ABX Comparator system, which I helped develop, has been refined during the 10 years of its existence by the suggestions of many audiophiles and scientists. Some hardware improvements of this system can even be traced to the pages of this magazine (Vol.5 No.5). Other inputs have resulted in the development of double-blind listening tests which require no switching. The reason for perfecting listening tests is to develop the ability to hear sonic improvements when they exist as sound, rather than as mere claims. To quote the esteemed J. Gordon Holt on the subject of double-blind testing, "The losers will be the dissemblers, the frauds, and those skilled in the art of autohype. The winners, ultimately, will be music and the rest of us who are interested in the maximal fidelity of reproduced music."—David Clark -------------------- Does Mr. Clark now consider you a turncoat, since you've in essence called him a POS? Then of course, there's this bit of POS: "The human brain is best at making sense out of nonsense. Humans tend to find differences and distinctions whether they exist or not. The research of Richard M. Warren, Diana Deutsch, and others, confirms that humans can decipher a word obscured by noise as much as a minute after a sentence was spoken (footnote 8). This same potential can, at times, create the wrong word. We can misunderstand and still believe firmly we heard a word different than the one spoken. This is not a defect. It allowed our ancestors to survive by detecting threats through noise. Sometimes they overreacted and called out defences when no mastodon approached. This did no harm". --------------------- Totally absurd, innit, Arnold? Taling about prehistoric mammals and all. What does *that* have to do with audio? Plus, everyone knows how short the auditory memory is. Who does this guy think he is, going against conventional wisdom like that? Once again, RAO should applaud you for alerting it to the horribly misleading POS that Mr. Atkinson has attempted to foist upon it. |
#18
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"dave weil" wrote in message
On Sun, 5 Sep 2004 05:08:52 -0400, "Arny Krueger" wrote: "Robert Morein" wrote in message "John Atkinson" wrote in message om... "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile John, Good article. It's a biased misleading POS designed to appeal to people who want to believe in magic, not science. From the article: The ABX Comparator system, which I helped develop, has been refined during the 10 years of its existence by the suggestions of many audiophiles and scientists. Some hardware improvements of this system can even be traced to the pages of this magazine (Vol.5 No.5). Other inputs have resulted in the development of double-blind listening tests which require no switching. The reason for perfecting listening tests is to develop the ability to hear sonic improvements when they exist as sound, rather than as mere claims. To quote the esteemed J. Gordon Holt on the subject of double-blind testing, "The losers will be the dissemblers, the frauds, and those skilled in the art of autohype. The winners, ultimately, will be music and the rest of us who are interested in the maximal fidelity of reproduced music."-David Clark -------------------- Does Mr. Clark now consider you a turncoat, since you've in essence called him a POS? The logical flaw here is that the presence of one or even a few true facts and well-considered opinions in an article does not prevent the entire article from still being on balance, a POS. I suspect that Weil would be able to detect this flaw in someone else's writing, but being highly challenged in the self-awareness department, he missed this critical point in his own writing. Note that neither Weil nor Atkinson quoted the following paragraph from the Stereophile aritcle on RAO: "Les Leventhal's critique of the statistical analysis commonly used in blind subjective testing is misleading, erroneous, and borders on the incompetent. His letter is written in a style that prompts the casual reader to think "Someone has finally figured out what's wrong with all those blind tests where they don't hear anything." Not only has Leventhal failed to prove his case; he has demonstrated his own lack of understanding of how the audiophile benefits from double-blind testing. " Dave was being a gentleman at this point and didn't call Leventhal's weirdness a POS. But he pretty well communicated the same basic idea. Apparently, the absence of a snapply comeback from Atkinson means that he reread page 5 of his own article. |
#20
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Jacob Kramer" wrote in message
om (John Atkinson) wrote in message . com... "Michael McKelvy" wrote in message link.net... This is excerpted from a thread on the diyaudio.com website I thought Arny might wish to weigh in either here or there on the matter. You didn't include the link to the Stereophile article on double-blind testing. It is http://www.stereophile.com/features/141 . John Atkinson Editor, Stereophile Thank you for posting this--it is very informative. I wish I had read this a long time ago. There are three problems here that I had suspected for a while. The first, which isn't the main focus of the article, is generalizability, which Clark conceded at the outset. Results are only valid for the two pieces of equipment under test. Period. I'm glad that you finally figured this out Kramer after only how many years of uselessly pontificating about DBTs? The second is the conclusion reached about equipment from a small number of trials. The clearest example of this was the conclusion that PAS and Yamaha sounded the same based on a handful of trials at Sunshine Stereo. Zippy repeatedly claimed that there were mind-blowing differences between these amps. All of a sudden you're haggling over the number of trials in the test Kramer? The third is the difficulty of identifying subtle differences in an ABX test. It's no problem at all, if the subtle differences are in fact audible. A further important fact emerged in this exchange, which it is interesting to see has been of such longstanding duration. And that is the tendency of this same small group of men to impugn the objectivity and integrity of those who criticize ABX, to claim criticisms are the result of not taking such tests, and to expound the virtues of ABX in whatever space available no matter what the topic at hand. It's not our fault that ABX critics are generally either greatly mistaken or just plain frauds. This has clearly been an ongoing multimedia gesamtkunstwerk since at least 1986. Ignorance of Clark, David L., "High-Resolution Subjective Testing Using a Double-Blind Comparator", Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, Vol. 30 No. 5, May 1982, pp. 330-338. noted. There is also a weird tendency to use nonsensical logic and nonsequiter in response to basic logical objections. Please see the collected writing of a certain Ludivic Mirabel. For instance Nousaine's interpretation of the probability of a difference being heard as a probability of a difference being present. Double-talk from Kramer. |
Reply |
Thread Tools | |
Display Modes | |
|
|
![]() |
||||
Thread | Forum | |||
Day in the Life of Joe Middle-Class Republican | Audio Opinions | |||
Facing subs towards driver or away | Car Audio |