Home |
Search |
Today's Posts |
#81
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bruce Abrams wrote in message ...
If you allow for the fact that sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences than you must control for it, always. Failure to do so leaves open the possibility that you may have been influenced by sighted bias. There would simply be no way to know whether the listening results were valid or bias influenced, and no amount of arm waving shouting "DON'T TELL ME WHAT I HEARD" will change that fact. Bias controls are necessary not because the biases always exist, but exactly because they may exist. I would like you to explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard in detail, not just in general. To claim 'you heard differences because you expected to' is not an explanation at all. It does not account for, for instance, the nature of the differences I heard (dynamic compression, brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as explaining fire by invoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing. |
#82
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"differences where very small. Perhaps expectation bias might have
clouded an otherwise identical verdict. But it is hard to explain his other descriptions as being based on expectation bias. As he has tried to point out." Even to have the expectation bias that amps often and routinely sound different is to set the base from which one then might easily start to assign differences of the kind he mentioned. It might even demand that differences be distinguished, which I have often thought is the plight of the "reviewer" for the hi fi rags who must fill up the page with something with which to keep them reading each issue. His was but a restatement of the untested assertion that unstructured testing reveals easily percieved differences, it is a bit of the dog chasing it's tail. That critter can be put on the straight again by doing the test, restatements of the assertion notwithstanding. |
#84
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Stewart Pinkerton wrote in message news:vcL0c.95418$4o.117983@attbi_s52...
On Sun, 29 Feb 2004 19:32:27 GMT, lcw999 wrote: Now, having said that, I am aware that some participants of this forum...long since committing themselves to a basic "...all sound the same..there is no difference..." mindset..will pile into this somewhat humorous fray. They will be very adamant in knowing what "you" can and can't hear! The humor of that stance comes to play when one can easily follow the logic that you or I, nor anyone knows what another individual's mental processes are doing to the interpretive processes. This is unique to each individual. The humour is however somewhat dissipated when you consider that what is being said is analagous to my stating as an absolute fact that *you* cannot run a mile in three minutes. This is precisely what is at question: whether we can hear the differences. You're simply begging the question. The point of course is that no human can do this, in the same way that there is *no* evidence that *any* human can tell apart two nominally competent cables (i.e. not comparing 8AWG to 28AWG or other such silliness). Begging the question. Perhaps, we have a group that missed their calling... ..neuro-research..or some study of the myriad of variables in the mental processes on the analysis of input from external sources. Indeed so - and there has been a raft of research over the last century into human hearing thresholds and acuity, all of which supports the notion that 'wire is wire'. Occam..... So, I respect your hearing differences...no arrogance here about what you do or do not hear. If one hears cable or amplifier differences..so be it! However, not one has been found who can do this when they don't *know* what is connected, so your 'respect' is rather misplaced. The simplest explanation to account for audible differences among components is audible differences among components....the burden of proof is therefore upon YOU, not me....and I am not satisfied with the mere assertions you have presented so far... |
#85
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Steven Sullivan wrote in message ...
If I gave you those 7 different amps to listen to -- the ones I listened to 17 years ago -- and if you could not tell any of them them apart, then your hearing is impaired. I cannot make it plainer. I understand you perfectly. Do you understand the how you might be fundamentally mistaken? I suggest it is the other way around. That is because no two sounded alike, and most sounded vastly different. This conclusion was confirmed by a friend who also listened to them, and heard the same things. And the flaws in such reasoning have been pointed out to you numerous times now. You have assumed what you should be *proving*. There are no 'flaws' in this reasoning. It was an observation. After going through these amps several times, I began to note which ones had a particular sound, and that sound was consistent from one trial to the next. Well, yes, of course it was. But alas that doesn't mean that 'sound' was real. A false positive effect of that nature is by no means improbable. Impossible, to be honest. The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. Then by all means, feel free to cease doing so. That's what killfiles are for. I don't btw imagine my replies to you will penetrate your resistance to scientific fact, which you've established firmly; I post them for the putative reader who might be following along, perhaps wanting to see the arguments on both sides. 'Scientific fact'? You call what you offer here as 'scientific fact'? You sit there behind your computer and tell me what I can hear? I doubt any scientist would approve of this methodology... I wonder if there is an aural equivalent to color-blindness.... If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. By themselves, these instructions do no describe a good comparative listening trial of amplifiers. Since I use the Stax headphones with the amplifier, it the ONLY test that makes sense. I'm the one who has to live with my choice: not you. |
#86
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Mon, 01 Mar 2004 23:11:08 GMT, "Harry Lavo"
wrote: "Stewart Pinkerton" wrote in message news:vcL0c.95418$4o.117983@attbi_s52... On Sun, 29 Feb 2004 19:32:27 GMT, lcw999 wrote: Now, having said that, I am aware that some participants of this forum...long since committing themselves to a basic "...all sound the same..there is no difference..." mindset..will pile into this somewhat humorous fray. They will be very adamant in knowing what "you" can and can't hear! The humor of that stance comes to play when one can easily follow the logic that you or I, nor anyone knows what another individual's mental processes are doing to the interpretive processes. This is unique to each individual. The humour is however somewhat dissipated when you consider that what is being said is analagous to my stating as an absolute fact that *you* cannot run a mile in three minutes. The point of course is that no human can do this, in the same way that there is *no* evidence that *any* human can tell apart two nominally competent cables (i.e. not comparing 8AWG to 28AWG or other such silliness). Gosh, Stewart, how long did it take you to test every human and every piece of wire ever used by them, and then verifying "competency" tests on those that might have sounded different, to prove you point. I don't have to, since all existing evidence and all medical and engineering knowledge, says that I am right about this. If *you* wish to claim otherwise, then that is an extraordinary claim, and the burden of proof is on *you*. Or might this be, just might it be, and assertion, a judgement, your considered opinon? It's a considered opinion based on a *total* lack of evidence to support the existence of 'cable sound'. Naw, it surely is a "fact". It surely is a good working premise - unless you can offer *any* shred of evidence in rebuttal. Perhaps, we have a group that missed their calling... ..neuro-research..or some study of the myriad of variables in the mental processes on the analysis of input from external sources. Indeed so - and there has been a raft of research over the last century into human hearing thresholds and acuity, all of which supports the notion that 'wire is wire'. Last I looked, this thread was about amplifiers and what Michael feels he heard in comparing five of them. Clearly, you didn't look at the thread title. Michael is claiming that he can't possibly be mistaken in what he thinks he hears in sighted tests, we are pointing out that it's not only possible, it's highly likely. So, I respect your hearing differences...no arrogance here about what you do or do not hear. If one hears cable or amplifier differences..so be it! However, not one has been found who can do this when they don't *know* what is connected, so your 'respect' is rather misplaced. Gosh, Stewart, what happened to the *properly designed* and *nominally competent* disclaimers? A few more opinions slipping into fact? Merely brevity - the disclaimers still apply to amps, but may safely be extended to *all* so-called 'audiophile' cable. -- Stewart Pinkerton | Music is Art - Audio is Engineering |
#87
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Harry Lavo" wrote in message
news:17V0c.436628$I06.4934600@attbi_s01... "Bruce Abrams" wrote in message ... *snip* quoted text If you allow for the fact that sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences than you must control for it, always. Failure to do so leaves open the possibility that you may have been influenced by sighted bias. There would simply be no way to know whether the listening results were valid or bias influenced, and no amount of arm waving shouting "DON'T TELL ME WHAT I HEARD" will change that fact. Bias controls are necessary not because the biases always exist, but exactly because they may exist. The are not *NEEDED* for home audio purchases and comparisons as long as the person doing the comparison is willing to accept some risk. And expectation bias as postulated has to be stretched to the extreme to cover five amps with four different "sounds". This is a specious argument, as no listening tests at all are *NEEDED* for home audio purchases and comparisons as long as the purchaser is willing to accept some risk. The bias inherent in sighted tests has been proven to exist (and you, Harry, have acknowledged its existense in other threads). The effect of such bias in a comparison of 5 or 7 or even 10 amps is no more unusual than its effect in a 2 amp comparison. This part of this thread started rather interestingly with Michael asserting that Occam's Razor suggested that the simplest explanation for people claiming to hear amp and cable differences was that the differences exist. Stewart and Steven both jumped in to claim that, no, Occam's Razor suggested expectation bias as the likely culprit. However, neither explained to Michael what expectation bias was. Surely you are aware that expectation and other sighted biases have been explained to Michael many, many times in recent months, and each time that they were Michael has responded the same way; namely to maintain that he is uniquely immune to such biases. *snip* for brevity Michael's type of comparative test is one many audiophiles have done for themselves at times..a shootout comparison. Not the most scientific. There's nothing in such a comparison that is remotely scientific. But there is nothing in such a shootout to assign sound character to the amps, as Michael points out. The worst expectation bias can do is to make one assume differences exist. A fairly significant point in attempting to establish a preference where none might exist, wouldn't you say? He has already said that for one pairing the differences where very small. But he decided that the differences were there none-the-less. I'm sure I'm not the only one who found it particularly difficult to discern between two cables, yet after many swaps decided that there were some very subtle differences. The brain wants to hear such differences no matter how loudly we protest to the contrary. *snip* And as I have tried to point out elsewhere, this "sighted bias" stuff is often used in a negative way and without any real consideration given to its applicability. In my opinion, it is often overused here a s a "club" and to show off. Educating people is fine. Picking fights or debates with them without even explaining your terms is not so terrific. One of the purposes of this NG is education. When someone approaches with a question regarding a cable suggestion, isn't it appropriate to suggest that they conduct their listening test in a blind fashion to ensure that they don't spend an extra $500 on cable when it would be far more effectively spent on speakers? For what it is worth, I am probably familiar with two of the amps he tested (by knowing the brand) and would agree with his characterization of the sound if they were the amps I heard. Not that that is definitive in any way, but it may mean these brands do have a characteristic sound and that he heard them. Or perhaps we are just two small parts of a mass delusion. The masses have been deluded regarding far weightier matters. |
#88
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
chung wrote in message news:AaL0c.95414$4o.117307@attbi_s52...
Michael, I and others have described similar tests here under similar conditions, and have always been told we are just imagining the differences based on "expectation bias". Expectant of what they can't say. Unfortunately, some of the members of this forum, while intellectually understanding it, have a difficult time differentiating between "sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences" with "sight *always* overrides true differences and makes your comparison invalid". This is a misrepresentation of those members' position. What some of us are saying is that you have to be cognizant of the effects of expectation bias, and take proper steps to control it , if you really want to find out if there are *audible only* differences. We always have said that if the differences are big enough, like those between speakers, then you don't really need DBT's to differentiate them. We don't say that "sight always overrides true differences" (in fact we argue if the audible difference exists in the first place), we are saying that expectation bias is very likely to override subtle differences, and that DBT is the best way to control for expectation bias. In the case of competent amps and speakers, we know that those differences should be subtle at best, from measurements like frequency response, distortion and signal-to-noise ratio tests. What am I 'expecting'? How can I 'expect' the Bryston to sound dull and the PS Audio to sound bright, when I never heard them before? How can I 'expect' them into sounding the same in repeated trials? They should know better, but they don't seem to be able to allow even the possibility that there are real differences and that you might have heard them. No, they do, that's why they recommend the Harry Lavo's and Michael Scarpitti's of this newsgroup to do controlled tests to see if those differences are real. Heck, they even throw in real money to motivate them, in the case of cables. The control of the test is valid. I connect A to Stax and listen. Then I connect B to Stax and listen. Then I connect A again and listen. Then I connect B again and listen. Then I connect C and listen, etc.... Given that that is how we use amps, that is valid for testing them...we hook them up and listen... I took all these amps home because I did not know anything about them. I had not been hanging around audio salons all that much to have formed any opinions on any of them. The only one of which I had formed any impression was the Harmon Kardon Citation, as I had read some of their literature. |
#89
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In Bruce Abrams writes:
"Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message news:ebL0c.156130$jk2.596671@attbi_s53... Bruce Abrams wrote in message news:wUz0c.91281$4o.116016@attbi_s52... *snip* quoted text How could my 'expectation' have given wildly different, CONSISTENT sound to each amp? Because you made your judgement the first time you heard it and then confirmed it to yourself each time you listened to each amp. When you engage in the type of uncontrolled, sighted listening that you did with the amps, you need ways of charecterizing the sound from each amp. Those very characterizations presuppose that the amps will sound different, otherwise you'd have exactly the same listening notes from each amp, and nobody really wants to admit to themselves that they heard no differences. Why did the Bryston sound rolled-off at the top? Irrelevant why it did to you at the time. When you sat down to listen to the Bryston you needed words to characterize the sound, as I mentioned previously. The point is that once you thought it sound rolled-off, you confirmed it to yourself each time by saying, "yup, there's that high frequency roll-off again," hence the consistency of the result. If you wouldn't have known which amp was playing the second time, you would have been listening to characterize it again and not to confirm what you thought you heard the first time, and chances are no better than random that you would have characterized the Bryston the same the second time. Yes, and the same problem exists when you listen to A or B in a DBT. That is, you assign a "label" to the A sound and then to B sound in the same way (i.e that A is "rolled off and that B is "bright") as you described above. There is no difference if you know that the amp is "Harman" or "A". So when you listen to X, you try to hear if it is "bright" or "rolled of". Since this "sound labels" where constructed in your mind, you have to obtain random results from this test, because neither A nor B nor X is bright/dull. The problem is that the results are random and they are random INDEPENDENTLY FROM ANY REAL DIFFERENCES between A and B, if any exists. This invalidates the whole idea od the blind testing. That is, the (correct) premise, that what we hear is influenced by not the sound alone, invalidates the DBT testing method. Piotr Wyrostek |
#90
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"chung" wrote in message
news:h1S0c.95108$Xp.423683@attbi_s54... Harry Lavo wrote: This is a misrepresentation of those members' position. What some of us are saying is that you have to be cognizant of the effects of expectation bias, and take proper steps to control it , if you really want to find out if there are *audible only* differences. We always have said that if the differences are big enough, like those between speakers, then you don't really need DBT's to differentiate them. We don't say that "sight always overrides true differences" (in fact we argue if the audible difference exists in the first place), we are saying that expectation bias is very likely to override subtle differences, and that DBT is the best way to control for expectation bias. In the case of competent amps and speakers, we know that those differences should be subtle at best, from measurements like frequency response, distortion and signal-to-noise ratio tests. What you are saying above is a very reasonable position. Unfortunately, it seems to believed only in the abstract here. When somebody such as Michael comes on saying he can hear differences in amps...there is no questioning him on his listening conditions Actually I asked him whether he level-matched... You did late in the game, but that was not the initial concern of those who responded. , no consideration of the age or circuitry of the amps in question (despite one being a digital amp...the one chosen at that). .no discussion of his stated purpose or state of mind. All that happens is that he is told because he listened sighted, he is surely imagining things. He did describe his listening conditions. Maybe you have missed that? Yes, and listening over stax headphones should probably have raised a few cautionary red flags among the objectivists, since listening on a really good set of headphones lets your hear things that ordinary speakers and room reflections might obscure. Then the turmoil ensues. The turmoil ensues because he refused to believe that expectation bias could lead to false positives when trying to detect differences. Now please answer this: do you agree with Michael on this key point? Do you believe that expectation bias should be controlled for? I believe it should be if you are after scientific proof. Done with a blind, monadic, evaluative test. I think it is a ridiculous burden to put on an audiophile trying to decide for himself what to buy...and it is an equally ridiculous thing to demand that he do before he dare discuss on this newsgroup the sound of products as he heard them on a "kitchen table test". They should know better, but they don't seem to be able to allow even the possibility that there are real differences and that you might have heard them. No, they do, that's why they recommend the Harry Lavo's and Michael Scarpitti's of this newsgroup to do controlled tests to see if those differences are real. Heck, they even throw in real money to motivate them, in the case of cables. Insisting on a test that the "testees" don' t believe is valid. Nice 'gotcha. Now Harry, how does the word "recommend" become "insist"? The insistance is that the test be a blind comparative a-b or a-b-x test rather than a serial, modadic, evaluative test that happens to be blind. As far as I am concerned, you don't have to do any controlled testing. You can pick amps/cables based on whatever criteria. However, when you want to convince others that there is real, audible, difference between them, you should use controlled testing like DBT to make sure that expectation bias (and other stuff like mismatched levels) does not invalidate your listening tests. DBT is the standard methodology on difference detection for such a long time, that I don't see any reason why Michael would have problem with it. He may not have a problem with it. Other than to consider it a waste of time. But he certainly didn't expect to be told that he was wrong, wrong, wrong to have thought he heard different sound characteristics from the amps because he didn't do the test double blind. He might or might not have heard such characteristics...he was given no benefit of the doubt. So don't get upset...it's a world view of theirs that you are not going to change. All you need to change their world view is to pass the cable DBT test! ![]() Sure, would greatly simplify the objectivist world-view here if we would just go away and stop challenging the test. The funny thing is no one asked you to go away and stop challenging the test. (In fact we even put up money hoping you would take the test.) And you were the one who said that Michael should go away and find some other topics to discuss... Nobody asked him to go away. We just suggested he not tear himself apart looking to convince you guys. It looks like he reached the same conclusion on his own. You yourself put the smiley in your statement, meaning you know it is a ridiculous statement that won't be accepted. Why, because you are asking us to "beat" a test that we think is flawed. Meanwhile, my efforts to point out why I and others believe it is flawed and to propose a proper control test have met with very little but stoney silence...as if the issue were never raised. If something uncomfortable comes up, just ignore it, right? The way to prove the test right or wrong is to devise the control test and get on with executing it. I say the best way to get to the other side of the fence is to open the gate. You say the best way is to lower my head and keep charging the fence. Which do you think has the best chance of getting us to the other side? BTW, how would you know that Michael would fail a DBT on amps? I don't know nor did I claim to know that he would. But like Mike Kueller, I believe the test technique itself loads the test in favor of "no difference". And I would never encourage him to undertake a loaded test. But you can ignore them and instead focus on other topics of interest here on the forum. I think the subjectivists actually find this topic of great interest, based on how frequently they post in these threads... You think the objectivists ever let an assertion of heard differences pass without comment or challenge? You realize how many of these threads were started by subjectivists? This one was started not once, but twice, by an objectivist. |
#91
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#92
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message
news:YDR0c.160642$uV3.708646@attbi_s51... The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. Then you will hear the differences. I don't deny that you heard differences. What I want to know is how you hooked up the headphones to the amplifiers? Norm Strong |
#93
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bruce Abrams wrote in message ...
"Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message news:YDR0c.160642$uV3.708646@attbi_s51... *snip* (snip) After going through these amps several times, I began to note which ones had a particular sound, and that sound was consistent from one trial to the next. And each time you listened to 'A' which you thought you found bright, you reinforced that it did, in fact, sound bright. Not so. I listened again, and confirmed that 'A' sounded bright AFTER listening. I cannot make this any plainer. Repeated trials confirmed initial impressions. The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. Then you will hear the differences. I've already heard exactly such differences between cables, right up until I realized I was hearing the attributes I'd ascribed to cable 'A', only I was really listening to cable 'B'. Until you allow for the existence of sighted bias, a phenomenon that is universally acknowledged to exist, you are correct in that further conversation on the subject is meaningless. How can 'bias' lead me to believe that two amps that I have never heard before. know nothing about, and have no opinion of, sound consistently different, that is, have consistent sonic characteristics from one trial to another and that mark them as different from each other? That is impossible, I put it to you. I had formed no opinion of 'Hafler' sound or 'Harmon Kardon' sound or 'Denon' sound. I had no idea what to expect. The Harmon Kardon Citation amp had the most impressive literature, and I expected this one to be rather good-sounding. It was not. It was rather disappointing, in fact among the very worst. The Denon was clearly superior. I had no expectation that this would be the case. The point is that I was judging only how they sounded. I did not allow the sales literature to sway me. |
#94
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#95
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
chung wrote in message news:AVR0c.160762$uV3.709405@attbi_s51... wrote: "not knowing what's being played. In fact, I would expect Keller to have had a better hearing acuity than most of us." Ms. Keller was deaf blind. Oops, didn't know that. So why would Mr. Scarpitti say that even she could tell the difference? Hmmm... Blind folk have the same hearing as everyone else, they only use it differently and pay more attention to sounds in a way sighted folk don't. In the case of testing audio gear, blind folk do no better then do sighted folk. Blind folk do pay more close attention to spacial clues based on sound reflection, which might mean they more easily detect it in recorded music or know it is false if reverb etc. are used to produce spatial information. They might be able to better comprehend the space a recording was made in, or know if it is a multi layered pan pot product. Yes, this was a way of saying that even a blind and deaf person could detect these diferences.... So, great, then you should have no problem passing a DBT then, correct? Further more, you are also implying that these differences will show up in measurements, too. |
#96
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
chung wrote in message ...
Michael Scarpitti wrote: "W. Oland" wrote in message ... The ability of knowledge and/or belief to influence how a person perceives something is well established. For example, that is the reason that placebos are used in the trials of new pharmaceuticals. Depending on what is being tested, up to 40% of the people taking the placebo report improvement in their medical condition (and also side effects.) This is completely based on the =expectation= that the drug will make them better. As such, the "real" drug under test must do significantly statistically better than the fake one. It cannot make a Harmon-Kardon amp sound consistently(!) different from a Hafler amp, especially when I have no idea how each is supposed to sound. It cannot make five different amps sound different from each other, and consistently so. The same thing applies to auditioning audio equipment, whether amps, speakers, cables or whatever. If you know which piece of equipment you are listening to at any given moment, your knowledge and beliefs about that item are going to influence your perception no matter how many times you tell yourself otherwise. I have said this before, and I am going to say it again, for the LAST time: I had NO beliefs about how these amps were supposed to sound. It was a 'blind' trial, in the sense that I had not listened to any of the amps before bringing them home for listening tests. Hafler PS Audio Harmon-Kardon Bryston Sony They all sounded different. How can my 'beliefs' affect my judgement, when I had no 'beliefs' to start with?! I listened with Stax electrostatic earspeakers connected directly to the power amps. Perhaps your system is not as critical. But DON'T tell me I cannot hear differences between amps this way. Hellen Keller could hear them! Several questions: 1. Did you level-match during your listening tests? I listened in succession to the same piece of music. I adjusted the volume as necessary. The differences I heard had nothing to do with volume. They were GROSS differences. 2. Do you think you can tell them apart in a DBT? I KNOW I could, with perhaps one exception. Two were fairly close, but the others were all quite different. The Harmon Kardon was similar in tonal quality to the Denon, but it had less dynamic impact, which was not noticeable until sharp, powerful bass transients occurred. Then it was obvious. If you played Mozart's soft strings on the two, it would be hard to tell them apart. But play Mahler's 5th, and it's a dramatic difference..... 3. Can you tell differences between cables? Yes, I can. BTW, whether Hellen Keller could hear them is irrelevant. Blind means not knowing what's being played. In fact, I would expect Keller to have had a better hearing acuity than most of us. Uh.....she was blind and deaf... |
#97
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message
... Bruce Abrams wrote in message ... If you allow for the fact that sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences than you must control for it, always. Failure to do so leaves open the possibility that you may have been influenced by sighted bias. There would simply be no way to know whether the listening results were valid or bias influenced, and no amount of arm waving shouting "DON'T TELL ME WHAT I HEARD" will change that fact. Bias controls are necessary not because the biases always exist, but exactly because they may exist. I would like you to explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard in detail, not just in general. To claim 'you heard differences because you expected to' is not an explanation at all. It does not account for, for instance, the nature of the differences I heard (dynamic compression, brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as explaining fire by invoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing. Michael, before anyone can answer the question of why you heard what you thought you heard, the question must first be answered, "Did you really hear it." This is the same question that consumer researchers must answer all the time and the reason that blind testing is the only testing methodology that is ever considered. What you're asking is the equivalent of being presented with a can each of Coke and Pepsi with labels in full sight, telling us you prefer the Coke and then asking us to tell you why you liked the Coke better. Until you can prove under blind conditions that you can distinguish between the two, there is no reliance on your initially stated preference. |
#98
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Piotr Wyrostek" wrote in message
news:7241c.443481$I06.4948340@attbi_s01... *snip* quoted text Yes, and the same problem exists when you listen to A or B in a DBT. That is, you assign a "label" to the A sound and then to B sound in the same way (i.e that A is "rolled off and that B is "bright") as you described above. There is no difference if you know that the amp is "Harman" or "A". So when you listen to X, you try to hear if it is "bright" or "rolled of". Since this "sound labels" where constructed in your mind, you have to obtain random results from this test, because neither A nor B nor X is bright/dull. If A & B do, in fact, sound different, I should be able to correctly assign "sound labels" with the repective amps and to then identify X as being either A or B. What is your point? The problem is that the results are random and they are random INDEPENDENTLY FROM ANY REAL DIFFERENCES between A and B, if any exists. The results are only random if there aren't any audible differences between the amps? What are you trying to say? This invalidates the whole idea od the blind testing. That is, the (correct) premise, that what we hear is influenced by not the sound alone, invalidates the DBT testing method. The purpose of blind testing is to remove the variables other than sound. If you don't know what amp is playing, it's impossible to prejudge it based on anything other than its sound, and if its sound is audibly different than another amp, you should be able to hear the differences and correctly identify X. |
#99
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#100
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"W. Oland" wrote in message ...
On Sun, 29 Feb 2004 20:18:11 +0000, chung wrote: You seem to have totally missed the point here. No one is arguing that Michael can hear those differences sighted. The argument is whether those differences are still there, if he does not know what is being played. Michael believes that there is no way that expectation bias can lead to differences, despite the body of research that indicates that indeed such biases exist and overwhelm subtle differences. Michael's viewpoint is being challenged here, not his ability to hear or not hear differences sighted. The ability of knowledge and/or belief to influence how a person perceives something is well established. For example, that is the reason that placebos are used in the trials of new pharmaceuticals. Depending on what is being tested, up to 40% of the people taking the placebo report improvement in their medical condition (and also side effects.) This is completely based on the =expectation= that the drug will make them better. As such, the "real" drug under test must do significantly statistically better than the fake one. You must also understand the drugs are tested on people who are ill and are not necessarily the best judges of the effects. ----------------------------------------------------------------- Here is a typical study, on Zoloft: ----------------------------------------------------------------- Table II Treatment-Emergent Adverse Experience Incidence in Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trials* ----------------------------------------------------------------- Percent of Patients Reporting Zoloft Placebo Difference Adverse Experience (N=861) (N=853) Percentage Autonomic Nervous System Disorders Mouth Dry 16.3 9.3 7.0 Sweating Increased 8.4 2.9 5.5 Cardiovascular Palpitations 3.5 1.6 1.9 Chest Pain 1.0 1.6 -0.6 Central and Peripheral Nervous System Disorders Headache 20.3 19.0 1.3 Dizziness 11.7 6.7 5.0 Tremor 10.7 2.7 8.0 Paresthesia 2.0 1.8 0.2 Hypoesthesia 1.7 0.6 1.1 Twitching 1.4 0.1 1.3 Hypertonia 1.3 0.4 0.9 Disorders of Skin and Appendages Rash 2.1 1.5 0.6 Gastrointestinal Disorders Nausea 26.1 11.8 14.3 Diarrhea/Loose Stools 17.7 9.3 8.4 Constipation 8.4 6.3 2.1 Dyspepsia 6.0 2.8 3.2 Vomiting 3.8 1.8 2.0 Flatulence 3.3 2.5 0.8 Anorexia 2.8 1.6 1.2 Abdominal Pain 2.4 2.2 0.2 Appetite Increased 1.3 0.9 0.4 General Fatigue 10.6 8.1 2.5 Hot Flushes 2.2 0.5 1.7 Fever 1.6 0.6 1.0 Back Pain 1.5 0.9 0.6 Metabolic and Nutritional Disorders Thirst 1.4 0.9 0.5 Musculo-Skeletal System Disorders Myalgia 1.7 1.5 0.2 Psychiatric Disorders Insomnia 16.4 8.8 7.6 Sexual Dysfunction- Male (1) 15.5 2.2 13.3 Somnolence 13.4 5.9 7.5 Agitation 5.6 4.0 1.6 Nervousness 3.4 1.9 1.5 Anxiety 2.6 1.3 1.3 Yawning 1.9 0.2 1.7 Sexual Dysfunction- Female (2) 1.7 0.2 1.5 Concentration Impaired 1.3 0.5 0.8 Reproduction Menstrual Disorder (2) 1.0 0.5 0.5 Respiratory System Disorders Rhinitis 2.0 1.5 0.5 Pharyngitis 1.2 0.9 0.3 Special Senses Vision Abnormal 4.2 2.1 2.1 Tinnitus 1.4 1.1 0.3 Taste Perversion 1.2 0.7 0.5 Urinary System Disorders Micturition Frequency 2.0 1.2 0.8 Micturition Disorder 1.4 0.5 0.9 ----------------------------------------------------------------- *Events reported by at least 1% of patients treated with Zoloft are included. (1)% based on male patients only: 271 Zoloft and 271 placebo patients. Male sexual dysfunction can be broken down into the categories of decreased libido, impotence and ejaculatory delay. In this data set, the percentages of males in the Zoloft group with these complaints are 4.8%, 4.8% and 8.9%, respectively. It should be noted that since some Zoloft patients reported more than one category of male sexual dysfunction, the incidence of each category of male sexual dysfunction combined is larger than the incidence for the general category of male sexual dysfunction, in which each patient is counted only once. (2)% based on female patient only: 590 Zoloft and 582 placebo patients. ----------------------------------------------------------------- Other events observed during the premarketing evaluation of sertraline: During its premarketing assessment, multiple doses of sertraline were administered to 2710 subjects. The conditions and duration of exposure to sertraline varied greatly, and included (in overlapping categories) clinical pharmacology studies, open and double-blind studies, uncontrolled and controlled studies, inpatient and outpatient studies, fixed-dose and titration studies, and studies for indications other than depression. Untoward events associated with this exposure were recorded by clinical investigators using terminology of their own choosing. Consequently, it is not possible to provide a meaningful estimate of the proportion of individuals experiencing adverse events without first grouping similar types of untoward events into a smaller number of standardized event categories. ----------------------------------------------------------------- The same thing applies to auditioning audio equipment, whether amps, speakers, cables or whatever. Unwarranted claim. No basis from generalization from drugs to audio. They are completely different sorts of experiences. If you know which piece of equipment you are listening to at any given moment, your knowledge and beliefs about that item are going to influence your perception no matter how many times you tell yourself otherwise. What knowledge? What belief? What did I know about Harmon Kradon, PS Audio, or Denon BEFORE I tried them? Nothing! I did not listen to these products before I tried them out. That said, there is absolutely nothing wrong with allowing your beliefs to enhance your enjoyment of music when using a particular item. Just don't confuse that enjoyment with the results of a double-blind test under carefully controlled conditions. They are two different animals. I have no 'beliefs'. |
#101
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard
in general. To claim 'you heard differences to' is not an explanation at all. It does not tance, the nature of the differences I heard , brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as nvoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing." Science is about defining principles which by observation and expeariment account for as much information as possible. What you are asking in principle is why the observed and confirmed byy experiment principle should not apply to your experience. In principle the questioned cann't be answered for an individual example. What can be done is to say that all things being equal, there is no reason to think this is an exception to the well observed and confirmed process of looking into such things over many years. To answer the question in a specific instance is approached as any other single example, apply the same test to it. If those percieved differences, or more accurately in this example, the ability to apply previously percieved observations to each amp when in a dbt will be any more then would random applications of same. In which case one can reasonably conclude that percieved differences assigned to each amp originate in some mental perception process independent from some basic nature of each amp. |
#102
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
chung wrote in message news:AaL0c.95414$4o.117307@attbi_s52... Michael, I and others have described similar tests here under similar conditions, and have always been told we are just imagining the differences based on "expectation bias". Expectant of what they can't say. Unfortunately, some of the members of this forum, while intellectually understanding it, have a difficult time differentiating between "sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences" with "sight *always* overrides true differences and makes your comparison invalid". This is a misrepresentation of those members' position. What some of us are saying is that you have to be cognizant of the effects of expectation bias, and take proper steps to control it , if you really want to find out if there are *audible only* differences. We always have said that if the differences are big enough, like those between speakers, then you don't really need DBT's to differentiate them. We don't say that "sight always overrides true differences" (in fact we argue if the audible difference exists in the first place), we are saying that expectation bias is very likely to override subtle differences, and that DBT is the best way to control for expectation bias. In the case of competent amps and speakers, we know that those differences should be subtle at best, from measurements like frequency response, distortion and signal-to-noise ratio tests. What am I 'expecting'? How can I 'expect' the Bryston to sound dull and the PS Audio to sound bright, when I never heard them before? How can I 'expect' them into sounding the same in repeated trials? I asked you whether you did level matching, and you have not responded. That is one major requirement on a listening test. As many people have explained to you, expectation bias comes in if you know that you are listening to different products. It is, to a large degree, subconcious. Your mind expects to find differences if you know that the products are different. They should know better, but they don't seem to be able to allow even the possibility that there are real differences and that you might have heard them. No, they do, that's why they recommend the Harry Lavo's and Michael Scarpitti's of this newsgroup to do controlled tests to see if those differences are real. Heck, they even throw in real money to motivate them, in the case of cables. The control of the test is valid. I connect A to Stax and listen. Then I connect B to Stax and listen. Then I connect A again and listen. Then I connect B again and listen. Then I connect C and listen, etc.... One more time, have you level-matched? What you are doing is fine if you want to select an amp. If you really want to know if there are audible differences, you need to provide control, as we have explained many times, unless the differences are gross. There is nothing in these amps' specs or measurements that would suggest the kind of gross difference that you heard (even Keller could hear that!). The existing body of knowledge in the field of psychoacoustics tells us that when detecting subtle differencs, expectation bias has to be controlled for, so what you are doing is not adequate if you want to detect those differences. Do you have any objection to the DBT methodology, which removes expectation bias? Given that that is how we use amps, that is valid for testing them...we hook them up and listen... Not good enough for detecting subtle differences. It is up to the individual to do whatever he wishes to select equipment, of course. I took all these amps home because I did not know anything about them. I had not been hanging around audio salons all that much to have formed any opinions on any of them. The only one of which I had formed any impression was the Harmon Kardon Citation, as I had read some of their literature. Explained over and over again already. |
#103
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Tue, 02 Mar 2004 04:30:57 +0000, Nousaine wrote:
Bruce Abrams wrote: "Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message ...snip to content ......... By the way, I asked a friend to listen along with me. His opinions were exactly the same. And he knew what he was listening to as well, correct? Here's another take on the "we all heard it" consensus. In the group open listening sessions I've witnessed the routine is interesting: First the Owner/Host/Presenter (they are nearly universally comparative) does a direct comparison or often a "comparison" with other products that aren't present, and asks "What did you think?" or, more common "Which Did You Prefer?" In direct comparisons there are often apparent level differences; but never is there a controlled attempt to level match. Further the O/H/P often primes the well with comments prior such as "we'll most people hear x,y and z). Next one ot two listeners express a comment and IF it's not the answer the O/H/P wants he says "Let's Try It Again with BETTER Program Material" and then repeats the process. If the "group" hasn't delivered the expected results this gets repeated UNTIL the 'right" or at least acceptable answers are obtained and then the presentation is finished. Listeners seldom say "they sounded the same to me" and there are often negotiations about what the real sound was "Well maybe you didn't hear the do-dah midrange but surely you heard the increased transparency...?" And eventually those who 'are' willing to speak will come to 'agreement' on what they heard; and then the experiment will go into anecdotal history that "everybody heard this." A number of books out there in the Scientific realm comment on how many projects are started based on "anecdotal" activity. It is this realm where an awareness level is brought to the attention of others working in a given Scientific domain and interest is raised. So be a bit wary of downplaying "anecdotal" notations. Unless "anecdotal" infomation flow is allowed many important facts will be squashed. Getting to the truth of a given issue is often assisted greatly by the much derided "anecdotal" information. Let me give you an interesting anecdote about this process. Clarke Johnson, an avid high-end audio retailer, and long time promoter of "absolute polarity" gave a paper at an AES Convention where he said that he'd done Triple Blind Experiments (3X-Blind, according to his interpretation meant that subjects didn't "know" that they were in an experiment) where 22 of 22 subjects reliably "heard" absolute polarity. At a subsequent CES show I was in a exhibit room and I saw Clarke expressing his beliefs about AP to a Conventioneer. I said that nothing he was saying had ever been verified under bias controlled conditions. Bias Controlled...you are going to control this? Every opinion you have has "bias" all wrapped up in. Enjoy your bias...you sure can' t escape it!! BTW..bias is not bad..contrary to modern day "political correctness"...it is based on your experiences regarding matters that required a mental "yea" or "nay"...it is part of your intellect. ...and can help you make a better decision. So he then announced to the room; "hold on everybody we're going to do a test" and he then played a 2-minute segment of an LP; walked behind the tower speakers and made a 'do' about doing something back there. Then he repeated the same music segment; and then asked "Did anybody hear a difference?" The guy next to me looked quizzical, shrugging his shoulders and then finally raised his hand when he saw a few others doing so. Then Clarke counted the raised hands and loudly proclaimed "See 6 out of 6 heard a difference." I then pointed out that I hadn't raised my hand (they did not sound different to me) and he conceded "OK 6 out of 7" totally ignoring that there were at least a DOZEN listeners present. Open social listening sessions often have the same interpretative error mechanisms; no data is compiled; negotiation between subjects is allowed, subjects who do not speak out vocally are ignored and only acceptable answers are accepted or acknowledged. Mercy..".interpretative error" compared to what..who defines the standard that qualifies an "interpretive error"..This stuff is getting a bit thick. There are variables in the "ear-brain" interpretation between each individual. Those out there that have a standard all figured out are to be commended... or perhaps the brunt of a few "guffaws". Perhaps, isolating parts of the mental process during the listening process will evolve into a new discipline...NOT!! Mercy! This is a bit much...reality where art thou?? This is heading into thicket of "illogic"...someone save us all from these mental distortions! As a group, some of them, browbeaten by those in the crowd that has it all figured out, others that merely comment on the positive or negative character of the component being listened to..so be it. Also, get rid of the "test" syndrome! This implication that somehow this "no data compiled" and "subjects make comments" are a dreadful sin. This is somewhat humorous. There will be an interchange of comments and ideas. With all the variables out there this is what the session will evolve into. This is good, one cannot compile any data that will not be somewhat different in another session. Surely, one recognizes this characteristic in this Audio domain! Unless they have a super scientific switching device that can shave the audio spectrum down to make everything sound the same. For those that need that...have at it! Perhaps the most humorous comment is that: "...negotiation between subjects is allowed...". Jeez, what was the purpose of the session? To arrive at a conclusion that won't vary in the next session? The "real world" is not a factor in this scenario. Do I detect an agenda, one that almost always clouds up reality? Then it is announced as a "test" that can be quoted in a browbeating manner? Happens on this forum all the time! Forget any ideas about "numericalizing" a social listening session or getting the same results over two sessions. Forget about setting unrealistic rules to tilt the results toward your view of the audio Universe. I repeat...one cannot allow for all the variables in each individuals "ear-brain" construct. You're chasing "hot air" to think that somehow one can force a listening session to adhere to some agenda that takes on an evangelist like quality. Then, go about quoting the results as a "test" that renders strength to your agenda. This will not work out here in the "real world"...one with this problem will have to gain sympathy from others of like persuasion...it is not in these newsgroups. Don't think this happens? Think about it. Try it yourself. (It is a given that the scenario painted above will give some kind of expected results...if not, badgering will occur until something preferred comes around..so what's new??) Yes, this happens...but one cannot fix all the sins you read into the lax rules...and, neither you or I have the answer with all the uncontrolled variables of the ear-brain constructs interpretations. Do try to enjoy the music...this evangelism in the Audio Domain seems a bit "out of place"...don't you think? To summarize...to attempt to isolate functions of your mental process to resolve an imagined problem in sitting in your own quiet listening space seems to drift into the realm of illogic. It also drifts into the problem area of "semantics"...ad infinitum. For instance ask 5 people what the meaning of "bias" is...note all the variables attributed to it. Who, in their right mind, wants to tread into this bottomless pit?? Leonard... P.S. A technique that seems to produce some results is as follows: A friend took a group of cables to a neighbors home and suggested that opinions be given about each cable...no cable names were given. Just a simple.." what do you think about this one." Then some suggested certain comparisons be made. No one knew what "brand names" were in the group of cables...just #1 thru #5. Out of this there was a general opinon that #2 was somewhat different in a rather pleasant manner. Some of the cables came from the mid-90's. The same session was held a few months later...the results were different. Case closed. Everyone left content with the fact that there are variables out there we do not have a handle on yet. We all left quite content that we didn't know the answer...but, we enjoyed it all..and went home and turned on our systems and made mental comparisons. We all knew there would be nothing definitive reached from the session...but we did hear an interesting cable or two and had some good audio chatter. No regimen here...no agenda..just curiosity to listen to varying cable characteristics. A good time was had by all. Some new CD's were played...the music was good... ..that is what this Audio domain is all about. Above all..there was an endearing respect for each individual's opinion..and some decent wine at the end of the session. Note also, there was no one there telling anyone what they did or didn't hear. A pleasant situation. Oh yes, at that time we were listening on a fairly new Panasonic S55 (Number?) that cost around $129. This merely confirms that these devices are reaching the "commodity" level. All the more interesting is that it played about every format on this Earth and seemed quite good at it. (See the a thread now running here, I think...about this Panasonic model) |
#104
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
(Michael
Scarpitti) wrote: If anyone says 'the fact that you knew which product you were listening to invalidates any audible evaluation you may have made' is ludicrous. Stewart Pinkerton wrote: No, it's a simple fact, easily supported by experiment in the case of subtle differences, such as among nominally competent amplifiers. Why are you so adamant in refusing to accept simple truths? Truth? It's your opinion stated as fact... I have never heard two amplifiers that sound the same - the greatest differences were between tubed and solid state amps - competent ones - and you deny those differences exist. Nope, your comparison method is faulty. The odds are that some of them did indeed sound different, but others did not. I can demonstrate a tube amp and a SS amp which sound obviously and vastly different to a room full of audiioophiles - and I won't actually have to change the connections for them to 'hear' the difference............. So what? Why will you not accept the plain fact that sighted comparisons are fatally flawed, and at least *try* it for yourself. What are you afraid of? Will your ego not admit even the possibility of error? Why not admit the fact that your application of the DBT *most likely* is fatally flawed. Will your ego not admit even the possibility of error? Of course it was consistent, when you *knew* which one was connected! It's called reinforcement, and any first-year psy student can explain it to you. This isn't new knowledge, nor is it even in dispute in medical circles, which is why double-blind testing is *always* used in medical research. Yes it used in medical and other research. But until a someone provides a *validation test* for open-ended audio component comparisons with music as the program - your strong beliefs are just speculation. The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. Not denying anything of the sort, merely pointing out that your comparison method is fatally flawed. Try it again under blind conditions, and you'll find that many of those 'night and day' diferences magically disappear. Those that remain are *real* differences, and you will have achieved something. At the moment, you are sticking your fingers firmly in your ears................ Can you spell *v-a-l-i-d-a-t-i-o-n t-e-s-t*? Regards, Mike |
#105
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
Steven Sullivan wrote in message ... Well, yes, of course it was. But alas that doesn't mean that 'sound' was real. A false positive effect of that nature is by no means improbable. Impossible, to be honest. So you have no aural imagination or have absolutely perfect control over it? That's like a machine, not a human being. |
#106
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Nousaine" wrote in message
... "Harry Lavo" wrote" ...snips to content .... I think the subjectivists actually find this topic of great interest, based on how frequently they post in these threads... You think the objectivists ever let an assertion of heard differences pass without comment or challenge? Try this: I, Tom Nousaine, analyzed nearly 2 dozen published controlled listening tests of power amplifiers that had been conducted to that time, of which only 2 (both high-output impedance tubed amplifiers; one of which was authored by Arny Krueger) had confirmed positives for audibility. Prior and subsequent to that time I've personally confirmed that more than a dozen amplifiers I've owned (or used) all sound exactly the same when operated within their power limits (Heathkit (2), Parasound (3), Stewart (2) , Fidek, Samson, Adcom, Bryston (4), B&K, SUMO, PASS and Yamaha) either to me or the several dozens of subjects employed under listening bias controlled conditions; even when conditions were maximally implemented to high-light possible differences. But whenever I mention any given experiment subjectivists never let that pass without challenge. The above suggests two possible conclusions: 1) there are no sound differences between amplifiers, except possibly between tube and solid state based on the output impedance of the tube amp, or: 2) the test used interferes with and confuses the normal ear-brain interpretation of music that audiophiles normally use, and therefore is insensitive to any but the grossest and simplest differences (eg. large two-dimensional differences in volume or frequency response). You and other converted believers simply ignore the latter possibility and have never attempted to validate the adequacy of your preferred tests. I and other subjectivists over the years have argued the case for the possibility that number two is the operative factor, and have argued for alternative tests, only to have it fall on deaf ears. Subsequently, most subjectivists have simple given up and gone on making choices their own way and living happily ever after. I and Mike and Wheel and a few others have chosen to stay, and stay vocal...but without letting it ruin our enjoyment of the hobby. |
#107
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
Bruce Abrams wrote in message ... "Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message news:YDR0c.160642$uV3.708646@attbi_s51... *snip* (snip) After going through these amps several times, I began to note which ones had a particular sound, and that sound was consistent from one trial to the next. And each time you listened to 'A' which you thought you found bright, you reinforced that it did, in fact, sound bright. Not so. I listened again, and confirmed that 'A' sounded bright AFTER listening. I cannot make this any plainer. Repeated trials confirmed initial impressions. The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. Then you will hear the differences. I've already heard exactly such differences between cables, right up until I realized I was hearing the attributes I'd ascribed to cable 'A', only I was really listening to cable 'B'. Until you allow for the existence of sighted bias, a phenomenon that is universally acknowledged to exist, you are correct in that further conversation on the subject is meaningless. How can 'bias' lead me to believe that two amps that I have never heard before. know nothing about, and have no opinion of, sound consistently different, that is, have consistent sonic characteristics from one trial to another and that mark them as different from each other? That is impossible, I put it to you. It's quite possible, ineed predictable from standard psychological principles. Your first impression is based on an expectation of difference; from then on you have the memory of what you thought they sounded like the first time. It's *possible* the amps sounded different. It's at least as likely , and arguably *rather more* likely, that they didn't. Your method by itself cannot resolve the question. That's due to simple facts of human psychology. I had formed no opinion of 'Hafler' sound or 'Harmon Kardon' sound or 'Denon' sound. I had no idea what to expect. The Harmon Kardon Citation amp had the most impressive literature, and I expected this one to be rather good-sounding. It was not. It was rather disappointing, in fact among the very worst. The Denon was clearly superior. I had no expectation that this would be the case. You misunderstand what is meant by 'expectation bias'. The 'expectation' in question is that apparently different devices will *sound* different. The point is that I was judging only how they sounded. I did not allow the sales literature to sway me. Even if that's true, it does *not* eliminate a fundamental source of bias: the certain knowledge that the component you are listening to at point B is not the same one that you listened to at point A. That;s *all* that's required to generate expectation bias. -- -S. "They've got God on their side. All we've got is science and reason." -- Dawn Hulsey, Talent Director |
#108
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
I would like you to explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard in detail, not just in general. If what you "heard" you only imagined, then we can't explain in detail what you "heard," because we can't know everything that was going on in your head at the time. What you *imagined* you heard could have been affected by the order in which you listened to the amps, for all we know. All "sighted bias" offers is a *possible* explanation for two things: 1) the fact that you perceived a difference among these amps at all; and 2) the fact that, once having identified a certain sound with each amp, your subsequent auditions confirmed those impressions. That this explanation is indeed possible is a proven scientific fact, and Steven Sullivan has suggested a few textbooks which will confirm that. A basic truth about listening comparisons is this: If you know what you are listening to, then everything you've ever heard, read or thought about that component can affect how you hear it. That's inescapable, my friend. To claim 'you heard differences because you expected to' is not an explanation at all. It does not account for, for instance, the nature of the differences I heard (dynamic compression, brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as explaining fire by invoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing. If you really want to understand this, may I respectfully suggest that you acquire some background in the science behind it. Sullivan has given you a place to start. bob __________________________________________________ _______________ Get a FREE online computer virus scan from McAfee when you click here. http://clinic.mcafee.com/clinic/ibuy...n.asp?cid=3963 |
#109
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Stewart Pinkerton wrote in message news:T%31c.23360$ko6.217138@attbi_s02...
Gosh, Stewart, how long did it take you to test every human and every piece of wire ever used by them, and then verifying "competency" tests on those that might have sounded different, to prove you point. I don't have to, since all existing evidence and all medical and engineering knowledge, says that I am right about this. If *you* wish to claim otherwise, then that is an extraordinary claim, and the burden of proof is on *you*. No, no the burden of proof is on you! I have two witnesses to a phenomenon that YOU say CANNOT exist. Prove us wrong! |
#110
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On Mon, 01 Mar 2004 18:23:23 +0000, Stewart Pinkerton wrote:
On Sun, 29 Feb 2004 19:32:27 GMT, lcw999 wrote: Now, having said that, I am aware that some participants of this forum...long since committing themselves to a basic "...all sound the same..there is no difference..." mindset..will pile into this somewhat humorous fray. They will be very adamant in knowing what "you" can and can't hear! The humor of that stance comes to play when one can easily follow the logic that you or I, nor anyone knows what another individual's mental processes are doing to the interpretive processes. This is unique to each individual. The humour is however somewhat dissipated when you consider that what is being said is analagous to my stating as an absolute fact that *you* cannot run a mile in three minutes. In the realm of "Ethics-Philosophy" this is akin to tossing out a known given...then attempting to spread all that "known" quality over the issue in question...it treads very close to what is termed a "silligism" in that realm of study. My stance is an absolute...yours is shaky..this all takes on a quality of immaturity. The point of course is that no human can do this, in the same way that there is *no* evidence that *any* human can tell apart two nominally competent cables (i.e. not comparing 8AWG to 28AWG or other such silliness). oops..a qualification slips out. Whup!...here comes the flow of "absolutes"..things like "no human" and the like...NO evidence. Gads...in who's book?..what mindset?.. ..this redundant flow of absolutes. Sorry, these newsgroups are full of this. A bit overworked...perhaps trite? Not convincing! Perhaps, we have a group that missed their calling... ..neuro-research..or some study of the myriad of variables in the mental processes on the analysis of input from external sources. Indeed so - and there has been a raft of research over the last century into human hearing thresholds and acuity, all of which supports the notion that 'wire is wire'. Granted there might well be research on hearing "thresholds" and "acuity"...but, this issue is not about thresholds and acuity....those studies had to do with detecting a frequency range upwards of 10 to 15k cycles...mainly in factories..I have discussed that issue with those that administered those test and one would not believe the variables in frequency range that first detection occurs...there is a lot of difference in the populace out there and their ability to hear even up to 10K cycles. (Hertz in the current vernacular). Just one of the variables I keep bringing up. Oh well...these facts are wasted here! So, I respect your hearing differences...no arrogance here about what you do or do not hear. If one hears cable or amplifier differences..so be it! However, not one has been found who can do this when they don't *know* what is connected, so your 'respect' is rather misplaced. Gads...we've missed the point, as always....it is not "knowing what is connected"..it is about one cable sounding a bit better or worse than another. Simple issue, but hard to grasp? Leonard... |
#111
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Bruce Abrams wrote in message ...
I would like you to explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard in detail, not just in general. To claim 'you heard differences because you expected to' is not an explanation at all. It does not account for, for instance, the nature of the differences I heard (dynamic compression, brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as explaining fire by invoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing. Michael, before anyone can answer the question of why you heard what you thought you heard, the question must first be answered, "Did you really hear it." This is the same question that consumer researchers must answer all the time and the reason that blind testing is the only testing methodology that is ever considered. What you're asking is the equivalent of being presented with a can each of Coke and Pepsi with labels in full sight, telling us you prefer the Coke and then asking us to tell you why you liked the Coke better. Until you can prove under blind conditions that you can distinguish between the two, there is no reliance on your initially stated preference. No way. The burden of proof is on you. I heard these differences as plain as could be. It sometimes takes a while, listening to various snippets of music, before all (or at least most) of the sonic character is revealed. They may sound the same when listening to soft female vocals, but then you put on something with big bass thwacks, and it immediately becomes clear that one has more 'slam' and speed than the other. That's why I would go back and listen again and again, to try to get a general impression of each amp using a variety of music types. The Harmon Kardom simply did not 'jump' when called for. It simply limped along, smoothing out the dynamics and making them soft and fluffy. Blind testing of drugs consists typically NOT of comparing two drugs, such as Zoloft and Paxil, but of a placebo and the genuine drug. The analogy to audio listening evaluations is not close. Do you know that the effectiveness of Zoloft and Paxil can be established, even though the mechanism of action is not completely understood? If the patients display and report improved mood, the drug works. Measurements of blood concentrations are correlated with mood, and that's all they can say. It is hypothesized that serotonin reuptake is blocked by these agents (SSRI's) but that is only an hypothesis at this point. Measurements of audio equipment are not comprehensive. They are cited simply because they can be measured. It's like the old joke about someone looking under a lamp-post for his lost key. You ask him: 'Where did you lose it?'. 'Over there', he replies, pointing to a dark alley. 'Then why don't you look over there?', you inquire. 'Because the light's better here'. So, when you measure signals in audio, you're simply measuring what you can measure easily: where the light is better. Whether all audible differences have anything to do with what can be measured is unknown. Till then, I'll trust my ears and not your pronouncements. |
#112
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
(Nousaine) wrote in message ...
"Harry Lavo" wrote" ...snips to content .... I think the subjectivists actually find this topic of great interest, based on how frequently they post in these threads... You think the objectivists ever let an assertion of heard differences pass without comment or challenge? Try this: I, Tom Nousaine, analyzed nearly 2 dozen published controlled listening tests of power amplifiers that had been conducted to that time, of which only 2 (both high-output impedance tubed amplifiers; one of which was authored by Arny Krueger) had confirmed positives for audibility. Prior and subsequent to that time I've personally confirmed that more than a dozen amplifiers I've owned (or used) all sound exactly the same when operated within their power limits (Heathkit (2), Parasound (3), Stewart (2) , Fidek, Samson, Adcom, Bryston (4), B&K, SUMO, PASS and Yamaha) either to me or the several dozens of subjects employed under listening bias controlled conditions; even when conditions were maximally implemented to high-light possible differences. But whenever I mention any given experiment subjectivists never let that pass without challenge. Without good sources and speakers, such a testimonial is without merit. The rest of the system must be of sufficiently high quality to make the differences, if any, audible. |
#113
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Harry Lavo wrote:
"chung" wrote in message news:h1S0c.95108$Xp.423683@attbi_s54... Harry Lavo wrote: This is a misrepresentation of those members' position. What some of us are saying is that you have to be cognizant of the effects of expectation bias, and take proper steps to control it , if you really want to find out if there are *audible only* differences. We always have said that if the differences are big enough, like those between speakers, then you don't really need DBT's to differentiate them. We don't say that "sight always overrides true differences" (in fact we argue if the audible difference exists in the first place), we are saying that expectation bias is very likely to override subtle differences, and that DBT is the best way to control for expectation bias. In the case of competent amps and speakers, we know that those differences should be subtle at best, from measurements like frequency response, distortion and signal-to-noise ratio tests. What you are saying above is a very reasonable position. Unfortunately, it seems to believed only in the abstract here. When somebody such as Michael comes on saying he can hear differences in amps...there is no questioning him on his listening conditions Actually I asked him whether he level-matched... You did late in the game, but that was not the initial concern of those who responded. , no consideration of the age or circuitry of the amps in question (despite one being a digital amp...the one chosen at that). .no discussion of his stated purpose or state of mind. All that happens is that he is told because he listened sighted, he is surely imagining things. He did describe his listening conditions. Maybe you have missed that? Yes, and listening over stax headphones should probably have raised a few cautionary red flags among the objectivists, since listening on a really good set of headphones lets your hear things that ordinary speakers and room reflections might obscure. Yes, listening via headphones is more sensitive. So is using pink noise. But even a bigger red flag is the alleged "huge" differences that even someone blind and deaf could discern. And his insistence that he was immune from expectation bias. Then the turmoil ensues. The turmoil ensues because he refused to believe that expectation bias could lead to false positives when trying to detect differences. Now please answer this: do you agree with Michael on this key point? Do you believe that expectation bias should be controlled for? I believe it should be if you are after scientific proof. Good. Let's remember that. Done with a blind, monadic, evaluative test. We had this discussion before, and you could not make decisions once you had to compare. Interesting that Michael told us he actively *compared* the several amps. He did not have any difficulty discerning differences in your so-called "comparative" mode. So why not do the standard DBT? I think it is a ridiculous burden to put on an audiophile trying to decide for himself what to buy...and it is an equally ridiculous thing to demand that he do before he dare discuss on this newsgroup the sound of products as he heard them on a "kitchen table test". You seem to have missed the point. If he said he heard differences between amps, that would be just an anecdote. What raised the discussion level up was his insistence that expectation bias could never affect one's ability to discern difference. He is perfectly welcome to his belief, but when he flat out refutes the existing body of knowledge on human perception, that's where challenges come in. Note that you have erected another strawman again when you said "it is a ridiculous burden to put on an audiophile trying to decide for himself what to buy". How many times do you need to put that up and burn it? Can you tried to be more objective in representing viewpoints of those you don't agree with? They should know better, but they don't seem to be able to allow even the possibility that there are real differences and that you might have heard them. No, they do, that's why they recommend the Harry Lavo's and Michael Scarpitti's of this newsgroup to do controlled tests to see if those differences are real. Heck, they even throw in real money to motivate them, in the case of cables. Insisting on a test that the "testees" don' t believe is valid. Nice 'gotcha. Now Harry, how does the word "recommend" become "insist"? The insistance is that the test be a blind comparative a-b or a-b-x test rather than a serial, modadic, evaluative test that happens to be blind. But we never insist that he has to do that in selecting components. If he wants to prove without doubt to us, of course then he has to use a bias-controlled methodology, and DBT is such a commonly used methodology. BTW, he did not think that he would have problem passing the DBT, unlike in your case where "comparative" causes panic. As far as I am concerned, you don't have to do any controlled testing. You can pick amps/cables based on whatever criteria. However, when you want to convince others that there is real, audible, difference between them, you should use controlled testing like DBT to make sure that expectation bias (and other stuff like mismatched levels) does not invalidate your listening tests. DBT is the standard methodology on difference detection for such a long time, that I don't see any reason why Michael would have problem with it. He may not have a problem with it. Other than to consider it a waste of time. But he certainly didn't expect to be told that he was wrong, wrong, wrong to have thought he heard different sound characteristics from the amps because he didn't do the test double blind. What he was told "wrong, wrong Wrong" was his insistence that expectation bias could not lead to false positives. You believe he's right? He might or might not have heard such characteristics...he was given no benefit of the doubt. I gave him the benefit of the doubt that he truly believes what he heard was real. So don't get upset...it's a world view of theirs that you are not going to change. All you need to change their world view is to pass the cable DBT test! ![]() Sure, would greatly simplify the objectivist world-view here if we would just go away and stop challenging the test. The funny thing is no one asked you to go away and stop challenging the test. (In fact we even put up money hoping you would take the test.) And you were the one who said that Michael should go away and find some other topics to discuss... Nobody asked him to go away. We just suggested he not tear himself apart looking to convince you guys. It looks like he reached the same conclusion on his own. It did not appear that he is tearing himself apart at all! You yourself put the smiley in your statement, meaning you know it is a ridiculous statement that won't be accepted. No, your interpretation is wrong. I put the smiley because I know that you or others know that you will fail the cable test, and will not participate. It is not because the test is ridiculous (why is it ridiculous, Harry, given night and day differences?), it is because the subjectivists are really afraid to learn that without sight information, they do not have that touted ability to discriminate. Why, because you are asking us to "beat" a test that we think is flawed. You said it's flawed because of that mystical comparative vs evaluative dilemma. Others said that it is flawed because the test period is too long, too short, snippets too long, too short, while in academics and industry DBT's are used day in and day out. Meanwhile, my efforts to point out why I and others believe it is flawed and to propose a proper control test have met with very little but stoney silence...as if the issue were never raised. Stoney silence? You seem to have very selective memory. If you are looking for posts supporting your proposal only, yeah, I guess that was stoney silence ![]() If something uncomfortable comes up, just ignore it, right? Please reread the responses to your proposal. The way to prove the test right or wrong is to devise the control test and get on with executing it. You seem to want to prove an established methodology is wrong. Yeah, go ahead and prove it. I say the best way to get to the other side of the fence is to open the gate. You say the best way is to lower my head and keep charging the fence. Which do you think has the best chance of getting us to the other side? Nil, because you made up your mind a long time ago not to go there! BTW, how would you know that Michael would fail a DBT on amps? I don't know nor did I claim to know that he would. But like Mike Kueller, I believe the test technique itself loads the test in favor of "no difference". And I would never encourage him to undertake a loaded test. Maybe because in most cases, there is really no difference? But you can ignore them and instead focus on other topics of interest here on the forum. I think the subjectivists actually find this topic of great interest, based on how frequently they post in these threads... You think the objectivists ever let an assertion of heard differences pass without comment or challenge? You realize how many of these threads were started by subjectivists? This one was started not once, but twice, by an objectivist. And subjectivists would never let an assertion by the other side pass without comment or challenge, so there you go... |
#114
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"If (in separate, isolated trials) seven test subjects are are left
alone in a room with 7 different amps and a Stax Lambda and SRD-7 connected to a high-quality source (I owned the Stax cartidge at the time, on a Magnepan arm and Thorens turntable), with familiarity of how to connect these to the amps, and after these subjects are given however long they want to listen to the set-ups, and all the test subjects report that the Bryston sounds dull and the PS Audio sounds bright, and the Harmon Kardon sounds kind of flat and lifeless, and the Hafler sounds kind of flabby, and the Denon sounds good in most respects but not outstanding in any, would that be good enough for you?" It is up to you to do the test, extraordinary claims require extraordinary support. If the above were in fact to happen just as you say, it would not at a stroke nullify the principle of expection bias. Before questioning it one would want to investigate why this example was the exception to the rule. For example did the output of the different amps interact with the transformer way shuch as to change frequency responce etc. in the headphones. We would also want to redo the test several times with other panels so as to eliminate the possibility of a random occurence of a pattern. It is possible to get 10 heads in a row in a coin toss. Only after those and other considerations were ruled out would the now established role of expectation bias be given another look. But to date, we have one testimony of personal perception and a suggestion to have others do likewise in the hopes of finding the bias is not supported, we have nothing else. It would be far easier to choose the two amps judged most different and see if in a dbt a pattern other then random would occur, all the rest of your proposed test with all the attached descriptive terms would not even have to be considered further. |
#115
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"2) the test used interferes with and confuses the normal ear-brain
interpretation of music that audiophiles normally use, and therefore is insensitive to any but the grossest and simplest differences (eg. large two-dimensional differences in volume or frequency response). " snip "I and other subjectivists over the years have argued the case for the possibility that number two is the operative factor, and have argued for alternative tests, only to have it fall on deaf ears." This claim requires you provide the testing for it's support. How does obscuring which device is active by covering connections with a cloth do the above? That you have failed to do the testing does not provide any support that it is valid,ie. to suggest alternative tests is not to undo the current well supported principle. Considering the above, how do you explain those cases where no actual switching was done but the other then "gross" effects were reported to be heard as easily as before? In fact, it is not more audio testing your theory above requires, it is a test about the current test so as to attempt to show it not valid. Because dbt is the standard in almost all research, that test would have to show why human hearing was an exception. You are tossing rocks at the proceedure of science, not the well established principle used in listening research and it's application to establishing the thresholds of reproduction which are audible.. |
#116
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"normanstrong" wrote in message ...
"Michael Scarpitti" wrote in message news:YDR0c.160642$uV3.708646@attbi_s51... The point is, it is simply not worth my time to converse with those who deny that such differences can be heard at all. If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. Then you will hear the differences. I don't deny that you heard differences. What I want to know is how you hooked up the headphones to the amplifiers? Norm Strong Are you unfamiliar with Stax electrostatic headphones? They are called 'earspeakers'. You simply attach the cables coming from the Stax transformer to the speaker outputs of your power amp. The headphones are polarized by AC current, and the power amp supplies the signal. They are extremely accurate and fast transducers. If you have a chance to listen to them, do so. |
#117
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Stewart Pinkerton wrote in news:aiq0c.3858
$ko6.13127@attbi_s02: On Sat, 28 Feb 2004 23:42:20 GMT, (Michael Scarpitti) wrote: wrote in message news:e4P_b.6984$AL.139593 @attbi_s03... Someone said they use extended listening in the dark to avoid light stimulation and do hear differences. The key is not the level of illumination but the knowledge of which is in the system. Prove it! There is NO evidence to prove that the senses are influenced by 'knowledge'. This is arrant nonsense. There is a *vast* slew of such evidence, starting with the classic 'false sighted' test where nothing is actually changed, but enthusiastic audiophiles duly report 'night and day' differences when they *think* that a tube amp or magical cable has been introduced. I too have observed sighted bias in a group of people as well as read reports describing the effects. In addition, it is the rare individual that does not succumb to peer pressure, arrogance, and other social factors which would then influnce the conclusion or reporting of his findings. r -- Nothing beats the bandwidth of a station wagon filled with DLT tapes. |
#118
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
chung wrote:
Michael Scarpitti wrote: chung wrote in message news:AaL0c.95414$4o.117307@attbi_s52... Michael, I and others have described similar tests here under similar conditions, and have always been told we are just imagining the differences based on "expectation bias". Expectant of what they can't say. Unfortunately, some of the members of this forum, while intellectually understanding it, have a difficult time differentiating between "sight *may* provide a bias that overrides true differences" with "sight *always* overrides true differences and makes your comparison invalid". This is a misrepresentation of those members' position. What some of us are saying is that you have to be cognizant of the effects of expectation bias, and take proper steps to control it , if you really want to find out if there are *audible only* differences. We always have said that if the differences are big enough, like those between speakers, then you don't really need DBT's to differentiate them. We don't say that "sight always overrides true differences" (in fact we argue if the audible difference exists in the first place), we are saying that expectation bias is very likely to override subtle differences, and that DBT is the best way to control for expectation bias. In the case of competent amps and speakers, we know that those differences should be subtle at best, from measurements like frequency response, distortion and signal-to-noise ratio tests. What am I 'expecting'? How can I 'expect' the Bryston to sound dull and the PS Audio to sound bright, when I never heard them before? How can I 'expect' them into sounding the same in repeated trials? I asked you whether you did level matching, and you have not responded. That is one major requirement on a listening test. After he claimed there was *no such thing* as expectation bias -- a claim I suspect even his fellow subjectivists find beyond the pale -- I posted some links to standard texts on sensory testing, psychoacoustics, and psychology, that support the reality of the phenomenon...and he hasn't responded to *that* either. Explained over and over again already. It seems clear that tutorial responses to Mr. Scarpitti cannot hope to persuade him of anything, but can only be of service to the gentle, and at this point quite possibly mythical, undecided reader. -- -S. "They've got God on their side. All we've got is science and reason." -- Dawn Hulsey, Talent Director |
#119
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Stewart Pinkerton wrote in message news:w241c.28038$PR3.503763@attbi_s03...
(snip) If you would like, go to an audio shop that carries used products of this kind, and ask to take them home. Hook them up to a set of Stax Lambdas through a transformer such as the SRD-7. Then you will hear the differences. Not necessarily, plus I've already pointed out that this is a *bad* method for comparing amps, since the load is not representative of normal operation. Since I actually DO use the amp to power the earspeakers, it is indeed a perfectly valid test, in fact the most important test. If it sounds bad with the Stax, I can't use it. You must understand listening over speakers is somewhat less useful, as the resolution is lower in speakers, and coloration of the room enters into it. If you want to know what an amp REALLY sounds like, connect it to a Stax transformer and earspeakers. |
#120
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Michael Scarpitti wrote:
Bruce Abrams wrote in message ... I would like you to explain how 'sighted bias' explains what I heard in detail, not just in general. To claim 'you heard differences because you expected to' is not an explanation at all. It does not account for, for instance, the nature of the differences I heard (dynamic compression, brightness, dullness, etc.). It is the same as explaining fire by invoking 'phlogiston'. It 'explains' nothing. Michael, before anyone can answer the question of why you heard what you thought you heard, the question must first be answered, "Did you really hear it." This is the same question that consumer researchers must answer all the time and the reason that blind testing is the only testing methodology that is ever considered. What you're asking is the equivalent of being presented with a can each of Coke and Pepsi with labels in full sight, telling us you prefer the Coke and then asking us to tell you why you liked the Coke better. Until you can prove under blind conditions that you can distinguish between the two, there is no reliance on your initially stated preference. No way. The burden of proof is on you. I heard these differences as plain as could be. Then why are you refusing to do a double blind test and hear the differences again in this test? Should be a piece of cake. What are you afraid of? |