Home |
Search |
Today's Posts |
#81
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Don Pearce" wrote in message
... On 15 Jan 2005 16:46:05 GMT, "Harry Lavo" wrote: My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight, and none of the arguments here have convinced me otherwise. Until the testing is verified not to interfere with open-ended evaluation of differences, and until the "disappearing" is investigated in more depth than it has been, I and others will continue to trust our basic instincts. Several years ago I conducted a test which the participants believed to be sighted. They all heard the differences quite distinctly every time, and had no problem at all identifying the two components under test. The only problem was that the cables I was changing were not in fact the ones carrying the signal. The real cables remained unchanged throughout the test. So without the pressure of blind testing, the participants were all relaxed enough to hear the differences easily. Shame really that there were no differences to hear. Yep...the first conclusion you can draw from this is that people don't expect to be lied to. The second is that people can easily imagine differences where none exist *IF* they have a reason to believe such differences should exist. None of this proves that people can't hear real differences sighted. Nor does it prove that blinding does not remove some real differences that are perceived under different listening conditions. This is why blind testing really works, and why the results it gives can be trusted. Yep, seems logical on the face. But not proven via controlled testing, especially as regards to something as slippery as open ended evaluation of audio components. |
#82
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"I guess there is no use in arguing with you...you have your mind made up.
But the size/capacity/stability of the power supply has a direct bearing, IME, on how well the unit presents the lower midrange and upper bass. It is one of the distinguishing characteristics between true high end sound and" And how will you support this assertion? More likely is the rule of ps enough such that any increase in capacity makes no difference. Either electrical measurements and/or blind listening test will be support enough for your assertion. I know, I know, it is a well worn sentimen in audiio marketing/publishing about the ps and how it makes a difference, show us in ways external to those claims that can be verified by others not so self convinced. The third party confirmation is how one gets around the you have a mind about this and I have a mind and we will not change it by continued assertion. |
#83
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Harry Lavo wrote:
wrote in message ... I guess there is no use in arguing with you...you have your mind made up. But the size/capacity/stability of the power supply has a direct bearing, IME, on how well the unit presents the lower midrange and upper bass. It is one of the distinguishing characteristics between true high end sound and mid-fi. It's not me you're arguing with as much as it is the blind testing I've been involved in. People are very "mental" about what they hear, or what they *think* they hear. When you know you're comparing a $5,000 CD player to a $125 player, and you're watching a sales person switch between the two, it's very easy to "hear" a difference. When you're forced to sit away from the players and can no longer see which one is being played, your ability to "hear" those differences simply goes away. I have very sensitive hearing, and enjoy high-end gear, I'm also very picky about my sound. And if I could find any solid evidence that a $5,000 player truly was better than a $125 player, I'd pay the $5k. But the difference simply isn;t there OR it's not a difference that your ears can pick up. You're partially fighting medical-science here, there are simply things that the human ear CANNOT detect. If my dogs ever learn to speak, I'll ask them if THEY heard a difference. =) My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight, and none of the arguments here have convinced me otherwise. Until the testing is verified not to interfere with open-ended evaluation of differences, and until the "disappearing" is investigated in more depth than it has been, I and others will continue to trust our basic instincts. Harry, have you studied what the partial loudness model is made of yet? |
#84
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out
many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight I don't understand what it is you're saying.Your ears and your eyes don't need to both be "on" to work, and blind testing is the most reliable way to find out wether or not we actually HEAR any differences between players. |
#85
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 14 Jan 2005 00:31:28 GMT, B&D wrote:
If you cannot give an example of a $5k player that sounds "as good" as a $500 one, then it would be good to just say so. There's been much talk here about the differences between $5k and $500 CD players. I'd be interested in hearing from those who think there isn't much audible difference. How low can one go with the low priced unit before you're not willing to make the same statement? I own CD players that cost me $9, $10, $25, $30 & $150. The $10 player (a portable) had many grave design defects; I consider it a failure. But the other ones sound identical--identical to each other, and identical to a Rega Planet to which they were compared. Not just to me, but also to other people with excellent hearing who were strongly motivated to detect a difference. Norm |
#86
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Harry Lavo" wrote in message
... "Don Pearce" wrote in message ... On 15 Jan 2005 16:46:05 GMT, "Harry Lavo" wrote: My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight, and none of the arguments here have convinced me otherwise. Until the testing is verified not to interfere with open-ended evaluation of differences, and until the "disappearing" is investigated in more depth than it has been, I and others will continue to trust our basic instincts. Several years ago I conducted a test which the participants believed to be sighted. They all heard the differences quite distinctly every time, and had no problem at all identifying the two components under test. The only problem was that the cables I was changing were not in fact the ones carrying the signal. The real cables remained unchanged throughout the test. So without the pressure of blind testing, the participants were all relaxed enough to hear the differences easily. Shame really that there were no differences to hear. Yep...the first conclusion you can draw from this is that people don't expect to be lied to. The second is that people can easily imagine differences where none exist *IF* they have a reason to believe such differences should exist. None of this proves that people can't hear real differences sighted. Nor does it prove that blinding does not remove some real differences that are perceived under different listening conditions. And what this response proves is that believers will find fault with any test that really IS a test. It seems to lead to the following equations: Blind or unknown = stress stress = no audible difference. Why either of these should be true remains unexplained. Coupled with the further proviso that "people don't expect to be lied to," and you have a situation where it is impossible to disprove any statement an audiophile might make. I've struggled mightily to come up with some valid way of testing claims of audible differences--a way that's persuasive to all audiophiles. I've failed. If it really is a test, there's always some objection. Norm Strong |
#87
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#88
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]() |
#89
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Harry Lavo wrote:
"Don Pearce" wrote in message ... The only problem was that the cables I was changing were not in fact the ones carrying the signal. The real cables remained unchanged throughout the test. So without the pressure of blind testing, the participants were all relaxed enough to hear the differences easily. Shame really that there were no differences to hear. Yep...the first conclusion you can draw from this is that people don't expect to be lied to. The second is that people can easily imagine differences where none exist *IF* they have a reason to believe such differences should exist. And isn't this exactly the problem with all sighted evaluations? Any visual difference, or any known difference between the units under test, establishes a reason to believe [audible] differences would exist. You can't reject Don's experiment on this logic without rejecting every sighted comparison you have ever done. None of this proves that people can't hear real differences sighted. Why would we need to prove this? Nor does it prove that blinding does not remove some real differences that are perceived under different listening conditions. Since when is it anyone else's job to disprove your baseless hypothesis? This is why blind testing really works, and why the results it gives can be trusted. Yep, seems logical on the face. But not proven via controlled testing, Tell that to the folks in the Psychology Department of your local university. They could use a good laugh. especially as regards to something as slippery as open ended evaluation of audio components. The reason it's slippery is that it involves not just sound, but also the mental state of the listener. The whole point of blind testing is to eliminate--to the extent possible--mental states as a factor. Designers of audio equipment should want to do this, because they cannot engineer for mental states. bob |
#90
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Stewart Pinkerton" wrote in message
... On 15 Jan 2005 16:17:41 GMT, B&D wrote: On 1/14/05 3:28 PM, in article , "Stewart Pinkerton" wrote: On 14 Jan 2005 00:31:28 GMT, B&D wrote: If you cannot give an example of a $5k player that sounds "as good" as a $500 one, then it would be good to just say so. Try the Meridian 800 series. Many would regard it as sheer engineering overkill, but it *is* designed to be utterly linear, without any 'high end' trickery to make it sound 'better' than mainstream units. OK, that is your $5000 player. More like $10,000, but whatever. What about the $500 one? Why mess about with a mere CD player at that price? Go for the Pioneer 'universal' DV-565, and get great sound from almost any variety of silver disc - plus all the films you can watch! If you insist on a 'pure' CD player, then the Arcam CD-73 is probably as good as it gets technically. I'm sorry, but I have that machine (568 here in the USA) as well as a Sony C222ES. On SACD in particular, there is a substantial difference between the two in the amount of transparency/ambience retrieval. I have never once put a SACD on, gone elsewhere and gotten busy and walked back into the room without being able to identify which machine is playing before I enter the room. I can do that with some other equipment. That doesn't mean the 568 does anything terribly wrong...it is actually a fine-sounding player....it just doesn't quite match the Sony in transparency. That's one of the things extra design money can buy you. The 222 uses Elna caps (second only to Black Gates in transparency) and separate power supplies for the digital and analog stages. Those are also the primary differences between it and the less expensive 775CE it is based upon...and side by side you can hear the difference. |
#91
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
wrote in message ...
"I guess there is no use in arguing with you...you have your mind made up. But the size/capacity/stability of the power supply has a direct bearing, IME, on how well the unit presents the lower midrange and upper bass. It is one of the distinguishing characteristics between true high end sound and" And how will you support this assertion? More likely is the rule of ps enough such that any increase in capacity makes no difference. Either electrical measurements and/or blind listening test will be support enough for your assertion. I know, I know, it is a well worn sentimen in audiio marketing/publishing about the ps and how it makes a difference, show us in ways external to those claims that can be verified by others not so self convinced. The third party confirmation is how one gets around the you have a mind about this and I have a mind and we will not change it by continued assertion. Yep, I'm still waiting for the confirmation of the test technique. Then I'll use it. Hasn't been done. In the meanwhile I'll continue to follow my instincts while being aware of the pitfalls. |
#92
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"chung" wrote in message
... wrote: On 14 Jan 2005 00:31:28 GMT, B&D wrote: If you cannot give an example of a $5k player that sounds "as good" as a $500 one, then it would be good to just say so. There's been much talk here about the differences between $5k and $500 CD players. I'd be interested in hearing from those who think there isn't much audible difference. How low can one go with the low priced unit before you're not willing to make the same statement? I own CD players that cost me $9, $10, $25, $30 & $150. The $10 player (a portable) had many grave design defects; I consider it a failure. But the other ones sound identical--identical to each other, and identical to a Rega Planet to which they were compared. Not just to me, but also to other people with excellent hearing who were strongly motivated to detect a difference. Norm I have a Pioneer universal player that costs about $130, and I can't tell that apart from another much more expensive player *after* I made sure that the output levels are matched. It is very easy to tell differences if they don't have the same levels, and I suspect that is what happens when you listen to them in the showroom. Mr. Lavo raved about a Panasonic player that sold for less than $100, so that's another data point. I raved about it, but that doesn't mean I can't tell it from other players. I commented on both its transparency and its concommitant "leaness". These are distinquishing characteristics that set it apart from many other players. I was most impressed with the transparency, as this is much more rare in inexpensive players than in more expensive gear. For example, the Arcam DV-79 @ $1500 has this same level of transparency but without much of the leaness. And therefore sounds even better. I also read the Stereophile review on the Apple iPod, and it measured very well against stand-alone CD players, playing uncompressed files. I suspect that there may be some detectible degradations in the cheap portables, though. |
#93
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 16 Jan 2005 16:32:28 GMT, "Harry Lavo" wrote:
"Don Pearce" wrote in message ... On 15 Jan 2005 16:46:05 GMT, "Harry Lavo" wrote: My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight, and none of the arguments here have convinced me otherwise. Until the testing is verified not to interfere with open-ended evaluation of differences, and until the "disappearing" is investigated in more depth than it has been, I and others will continue to trust our basic instincts. Several years ago I conducted a test which the participants believed to be sighted. They all heard the differences quite distinctly every time, and had no problem at all identifying the two components under test. The only problem was that the cables I was changing were not in fact the ones carrying the signal. The real cables remained unchanged throughout the test. So without the pressure of blind testing, the participants were all relaxed enough to hear the differences easily. Shame really that there were no differences to hear. Yep...the first conclusion you can draw from this is that people don't expect to be lied to. The second is that people can easily imagine differences where none exist *IF* they have a reason to believe such differences should exist. You miss the point. There was nothing to stop any of these people saying "I can't hear a difference". There was no requirement put on them to hear any kind of difference. Simply put - they expected to, so they did. None of this proves that people can't hear real differences sighted. Of course you can hear real differences sighted. What it proves is that you can hear non-existant differences sighted as well. Nor does it prove that blinding does not remove some real differences that are perceived under different listening conditions. This is why blind testing really works, and why the results it gives can be trusted. Yep, seems logical on the face. But not proven via controlled testing, especially as regards to something as slippery as open ended evaluation of audio components. The real conclusion is that you can't trust a sighted test. The results will be whatever you want them to be. d Pearce Consulting http://www.pearce.uk.com |
#94
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article ,
" wrote: You didn't expect start an argument, but you wanted to see if anyone thinks there's a difference so you can tell them they're wrong? Umm...no. I wanted to see if anyone could actually deny the reality of the situation and come up with any evidence to support their claims. The "I thought I heard it, so it's there" rhetoric is faulty. think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? I wouldn't say that you're "wrong", I'd say that you're hearing things the way you want to hear them, and not the way they actually ARE. Has anyone here been able to id the Arcam CD 23 blind? Stephen |
#97
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article ,
" wrote: You didn't expect start an argument, but you wanted to see if anyone thinks there's a difference so you can tell them they're wrong? Umm...no. I wanted to see if anyone could actually deny the reality of the situation and come up with any evidence to support their claims. The "I thought I heard it, so it's there" rhetoric is faulty. You'll notice I didn't use that rhetoric. think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? I wouldn't say that you're "wrong", I'd say that you're hearing things the way you want to hear them, and not the way they actually ARE. Why would I care? I own both units and use each for different purposes. Stephen |
#98
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
wrote in message ...
My basic problem is I believe the blind testing itself knocks out many significant "ways" of hearing beyond just sight I don't understand what it is you're saying.Your ears and your eyes don't need to both be "on" to work, and blind testing is the most reliable way to find out wether or not we actually HEAR any differences between players. I am saying that blind testing has not been verified as to its validity for hearing some of the subtle differences audiophiles seem to notice. Nor is this just wishful thinking. The fact is: hearing is a combination of physical propagation and complex brain evaluation. Plenty of studies show that different awareness states and emotional states affect which parts / how the brain interprets data. When evaluating components, we are trying to judge "musical accuracy" against some remembered archetypes of what live music sounds like. It is not at all impossible that when forced into a comparative mode the brain evaluates sound differently than in a relaxed, listening mode. It is not at all impossible that such things as a sense of ultimate transparency, a sense that all is right or not in how equipment handles micro- and macro- dynamics, etc. may be affected by the test itself. We do know that the brain is "hard wired" in some cases to evaluate music; we do not know whether this "hard wiring" is overcome by other senses in doing comparative evaluation. We do know that blind comparative testing can handle static loudness differences, static frequency response differences. That is all we really know. The validation for use of these test techniques, borrowed from eudiometry, for general open-ended evaluation of audio gear has not been done. The assumption that it is valid is just that...an assumption. And the typical response to these objections is that validation is not needed, since loudness and frequency response explain everything anyway. I for one do not buy it...until such time as the testing has been specifically validated with regard to these concerns, I and many others will simply follow our instincts that not everything we hear is an illusion. |
#99
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
wrote in message ...
Harry Lavo wrote: "Don Pearce" wrote in message ... The only problem was that the cables I was changing were not in fact the ones carrying the signal. The real cables remained unchanged throughout the test. So without the pressure of blind testing, the participants were all relaxed enough to hear the differences easily. Shame really that there were no differences to hear. Yep...the first conclusion you can draw from this is that people don't expect to be lied to. The second is that people can easily imagine differences where none exist *IF* they have a reason to believe such differences should exist. And isn't this exactly the problem with all sighted evaluations? Any visual difference, or any known difference between the units under test, establishes a reason to believe [audible] differences would exist. You can't reject Don's experiment on this logic without rejecting every sighted comparison you have ever done. Yes, except there is a big difference when evaluating, lets say, a known expensive and well-regarded piece of equipment vs. a cheapo, versus trying to ascertain differences between two different pieces of gear that are roughly of same value and manufacturing quality. There may be a bias towards hearing differences where none exist, but that bias is not likely to be so strong as to override an ability to hear real differences, if that is the goal. As a specific example, I recently bought a used power amp by a manufacturer based on my satisfaction with another piece of gear from the same range by that same manufactur. It was hopefully to replace a piece of gear that I had been relatively happy with, but I felt was slightly lacking in a specific regard. When I got the unit, I pulled out a few "test" disks and substitued the units back and forth, playing and replaying sections from the disks. My overall evaluation was that the units sounded essentially alike in frequency response and speaker control, and the new unit had the characteristic I had been looking for (also good). So I was predisposed to keep/like the unit. I put it in the system and used it as I worked at the computer for a week...but I noticed that I became tired of listening and slightly irritated after several hours..that had never happened with the old unit. Switched it back in, went another week, no problem. Put the "new" unit back in, another few days...same irritation problem. Back in went the old...no problem..and it is staying there and I am selling the new unit. If anything my expectation bias was that I would like the new unit, and the comparative testing tended to support this. But clearly long term there is a problem and it is a piece of gear I cannot live with. If I had "blind" tested, I might have concluded the units sounded the same (they were that close) or that the new unit sounded slightly different (and probably better) since it clearly has the characteristic I was after. It clearly would not have shown anything about the long term irritation that has caused me to reject the unit. None of this proves that people can't hear real differences sighted. Why would we need to prove this? Because there is a tendency here to assume that anybody who hears differences is kidding themselves, and that most likely there is no difference unless we are talking about phono cartridges or loudspeakers. That "standard" is applied to turntables, tonearms, tuners, CD players, amplifiers almost without discrimination. Nor does it prove that blinding does not remove some real differences that are perceived under different listening conditions. Since when is it anyone else's job to disprove your baseless hypothesis? Well, you haven't proved it baseless. But you don't have to prove it, and I don't have to accept your preference for blind testing. This is why blind testing really works, and why the results it gives can be trusted. Yep, seems logical on the face. But not proven via controlled testing, Tell that to the folks in the Psychology Department of your local university. They could use a good laugh. Come on. We've gone over this before. Nothing proven that specifically applies to some of the esoterica of audio component evaluation. especially as regards to something as slippery as open ended evaluation of audio components. The reason it's slippery is that it involves not just sound, but also the mental state of the listener. The whole point of blind testing is to eliminate--to the extent possible--mental states as a factor. Designers of audio equipment should want to do this, because they cannot engineer for mental states. Nice if you ignore the fact that the test itself changes the mental state. |
#100
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"MINe 109" wrote in message
... In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. The Arcam has a more advanced DAC and a heftier power supply, plus it's in a silver box instead of a brown one. And while we're at it, how do you know you aren't just imaging a difference between them? Happens all the time. Frequency response is likely different. Hearing "Elephant Talk" on the AMC was akin to hearing a completely different mix. In general the newer Arcams seem to have a "house sound" that favors a wide soundstage, bright and clear transparency, extended bass, but a slightly lean mid-bass. This sets them slightly apart from most CD players that sound a bit "darker". Why this is so I do not know. |
#101
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Ah, but that's what most people who buy those magazines want to read.
Atkinson and Pearson would starve if they tried to satisfy the likes of you! Well then I guess the biggest difference between me and most people is that I don't need a magazine reviewer to do my ear's job for me. I can tell what sounds like what all on my own. |
#102
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
hasenpfeffer wrote:
I have never quite understood the philosophy behind $5K players. They usually are very heavy, shock absorption everywhere, heavy heavy transport, solid mechanisms, exotic materials and what have you. A lot of money is being spent on a superior design with, as far as I can tell, the goal to read the CD without any bit errors. And that's fine. But, wouldn't it be cheaper and better to take a 48x CD rom drive, read the material a few times as soon as the CD is inserted, compare the digital data, error check and what have you, store the data in memory which is not prone to errors due to vibrations and play it back from a memory buffer? Especially when taking a fast CD Rom, the data can be read many times and compared and checked and errors can be eliminated while the CD is playing. No need to have a real time stream that can have errors directly from the optical pick-up element in the CD player to the output of the DAC. The player could even let you know exactly when there is a read-out error on the CD that can't be corrected. I would say a design like that is superior to an on-the-fly processing type CD player, and can achieve lower bit error rates, most likely completely eliminating errors while under $1K, even with enough RAM to store the entire CD content. This is what ExactAudioCopy does, a free downloadable PC-program. To find the right settings I experimented with read speeds and error correction by multiple reading. My results with a cheap AOpen52x burner (27 Euro) were very promising. Even at maximum speed single read, on almost all CDs that i tried, there was not a single bit different from 4x read at 4times speed, so there doesn't seem the need for multiple reads. One home-burnt CDRom of bad quality media would make a difference, in the fast read mode there were several "jumps" and muted passages, 3 titles were drpped etc., which still came out on the paranoid setting. -- ciao Ban Bordighera, Italy |
#103
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Harry Lavo wrote:
"chung" wrote in message ... wrote: On 14 Jan 2005 00:31:28 GMT, B&D wrote: If you cannot give an example of a $5k player that sounds "as good" as a $500 one, then it would be good to just say so. There's been much talk here about the differences between $5k and $500 CD players. I'd be interested in hearing from those who think there isn't much audible difference. How low can one go with the low priced unit before you're not willing to make the same statement? I own CD players that cost me $9, $10, $25, $30 & $150. The $10 player (a portable) had many grave design defects; I consider it a failure. But the other ones sound identical--identical to each other, and identical to a Rega Planet to which they were compared. Not just to me, but also to other people with excellent hearing who were strongly motivated to detect a difference. Norm I have a Pioneer universal player that costs about $130, and I can't tell that apart from another much more expensive player *after* I made sure that the output levels are matched. It is very easy to tell differences if they don't have the same levels, and I suspect that is what happens when you listen to them in the showroom. Mr. Lavo raved about a Panasonic player that sold for less than $100, so that's another data point. I raved about it, but that doesn't mean I can't tell it from other players. I commented on both its transparency and its concommitant "leaness". These are distinquishing characteristics that set it apart from many other players. I was most impressed with the transparency, as this is much more rare in inexpensive players than in more expensive gear. For example, the Arcam DV-79 @ $1500 has this same level of transparency but without much of the leaness. And therefore sounds even better. Sorry, I forgot that every CD player sounds different to you ![]() On the other hand, the fact that you raved about a $100 player means that it is possible to implement a good power supply in an inexpensive player, no? So you really can't say that the difference between "high-end" and "mid-fi" is in the power supply which shows up in "upper bass or lower midrange", since obviously you find the $100 Panasonic as good or better than some much more expensive players. I also read the Stereophile review on the Apple iPod, and it measured very well against stand-alone CD players, playing uncompressed files. I suspect that there may be some detectible degradations in the cheap portables, though. |
#104
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
MINe 109 wrote:
I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? Who knows? What design differences could be relevant to the sound differences you claim to hear? If one unit has gross FR variations or audible and unpleasant distortion artifacts due to its design then I guess you could be correct in your thinking. I have heard tell of designs where the idea was to roll off high frequencies, but I'm guessing that very few CD players exhibit this kind of gross anomaly. In any case, anyone paying extra for missing highs is being cheated; they'd be better off with something cheaper and, instead, using tone controls if they have them. On the other hand, I once read a review in one of the 'high-end' mags: an amplifier where the 'designer' claimed that changing a bit of internal wiring on the capacitors (from a few inches of Monster Cable to a few inches of something else) made a 'big' difference in the amp's 'sound'. If that is an example of the kind of design difference you mean then I'd say you are probably wrong in your thinking. michael |
#105
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Harry Lavo wrote:
snip Because there is a tendency here to assume that anybody who hears differences is kidding themselves, and that most likely there is no difference unless we are talking about phono cartridges or loudspeakers. That "standard" is applied to turntables, tonearms, tuners, CD players, amplifiers almost without discrimination. Not at all true. Turntables and tuners can often be distinguished. Amps and CDPs generally not, but that doesn't mean that people who claim to hear differences between them are kidding themselves. It takes careful level-matching to make two such units sound identical, and I'll bet most audiophiles don't whip out a voltmeter every time they try out a new component. So the differences they hear are real; they just aren't reflective of the sound quality of the two components. bob |
#106
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
MINe 109 wrote:
In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. In that case, you're on shaky ground ascribing the difference to "design," aren't you? The Arcam has a more advanced DAC and a heftier power supply, plus it's in a silver box instead of a brown one. It's the silver that does it, you know! And while we're at it, how do you know you aren't just imaging a difference between them? Happens all the time. Frequency response is likely different. Hearing "Elephant Talk" on the AMC was akin to hearing a completely different mix. In that case, you should definitely junk it. There's no excuse for FR problems in a CD player. bob |
#107
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
Another possibility is that he is hearing a real difference--the
result of differences in output levels between the two units. If one is even imperceptibly louder than the other, this could easily affect his preference for one over the other. Although I can't deny this as a possibility, I've heard enough units to believe that there isn't any difference as far as levels are concerned between any 2 players for there to be an overtly audible difference between the two. I can't prove this, but I've often suspected that audiophiles subconsciously tweak the volume control to favor the unit they "want" to sound better. Well, if someone is "tweaking" equipment somehwere along the line than it invalidates the test, that's as good as cheating. |
#108
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
And isn't this exactly the problem with all sighted evaluations? Any
visual difference, or any known difference between the units under test, establishes a reason to believe [audible] differences would exist. This is what I found. Regardless of which connections we were using (analof or optical), when we switched between players without the listeners knowing which players we were using, they couldn't identify ANY sonic differences. During a 2nd phase, we told the people which player we were using, and they suddenly developed the ability to detect differences because they knew when we were using a more expensive player. During a third phase, we kept the listeners blind, and would start with the least expensive player...we would then tell the people that we were switching to a considerablly more expensive player, but would in fact simply re-start the SAME unit, and they a few of them claimed to hear differences! That was the breaker for me, it became plain and obvious that people were "hearing" differences when they knew, or at least *thought* that we had switched players. And isn't this exactly the problem with all sighted evaluations? Any visual difference, or any known difference between the units under test, establishes a reason to believe [audible] differences would exist. And if you're the type of person who's mental status always makes it seem as though the more expensive unit is really producing better sound, then you'll always want the more expensive unit. I'm personally willing to except the limitations of the human ear. |
#109
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article , wrote:
MINe 109 wrote: In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. In that case, you're on shaky ground ascribing the difference to "design," aren't you? I assume pianos are built to different designs without knowing how to build one. The Arcam has a more advanced DAC and a heftier power supply, plus it's in a silver box instead of a brown one. It's the silver that does it, you know! But of course. And while we're at it, how do you know you aren't just imaging a difference between them? Happens all the time. Frequency response is likely different. Hearing "Elephant Talk" on the AMC was akin to hearing a completely different mix. In that case, you should definitely junk it. There's no excuse for FR problems in a CD player. I've been using it as a transport in another system instead. Stephen |
#110
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article ,
"Harry Lavo" wrote: "MINe 109" wrote in message ... In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. The Arcam has a more advanced DAC and a heftier power supply, plus it's in a silver box instead of a brown one. And while we're at it, how do you know you aren't just imaging a difference between them? Happens all the time. Frequency response is likely different. Hearing "Elephant Talk" on the AMC was akin to hearing a completely different mix. In general the newer Arcams seem to have a "house sound" that favors a wide soundstage, bright and clear transparency, extended bass, but a slightly lean mid-bass. This sets them slightly apart from most CD players that sound a bit "darker". Why this is so I do not know. The AMC had more "whomp" in the mid-bass, but generally seemed more shut in and less transparent. I wonder if leanness in one and a bump in the other coincided to make for a bigger seeming difference. Stephen |
#111
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 18 Jan 2005 02:22:07 GMT, "
wrote: Another possibility is that he is hearing a real difference--the result of differences in output levels between the two units. If one is even imperceptibly louder than the other, this could easily affect his preference for one over the other. Although I can't deny this as a possibility, I've heard enough units to believe that there isn't any difference as far as levels are concerned between any 2 players for there to be an overtly audible difference between the two. Your belief is without substance. It is well known that, while 2V rms is the 'standard' output for CD players, real values from player to player vary by up to 6dB. It is generally acknowledged that a 0.5dB difference will be heard by most listeners - but as a difference in *quality*, not loudness. -- Stewart Pinkerton | Music is Art - Audio is Engineering |
#112
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
"Chung" wrote in message
... Harry Lavo wrote: "chung" wrote in message ... wrote: On 14 Jan 2005 00:31:28 GMT, B&D wrote: If you cannot give an example of a $5k player that sounds "as good" as a $500 one, then it would be good to just say so. There's been much talk here about the differences between $5k and $500 CD players. I'd be interested in hearing from those who think there isn't much audible difference. How low can one go with the low priced unit before you're not willing to make the same statement? I own CD players that cost me $9, $10, $25, $30 & $150. The $10 player (a portable) had many grave design defects; I consider it a failure. But the other ones sound identical--identical to each other, and identical to a Rega Planet to which they were compared. Not just to me, but also to other people with excellent hearing who were strongly motivated to detect a difference. Norm I have a Pioneer universal player that costs about $130, and I can't tell that apart from another much more expensive player *after* I made sure that the output levels are matched. It is very easy to tell differences if they don't have the same levels, and I suspect that is what happens when you listen to them in the showroom. Mr. Lavo raved about a Panasonic player that sold for less than $100, so that's another data point. I raved about it, but that doesn't mean I can't tell it from other players. I commented on both its transparency and its concommitant "leaness". These are distinquishing characteristics that set it apart from many other players. I was most impressed with the transparency, as this is much more rare in inexpensive players than in more expensive gear. For example, the Arcam DV-79 @ $1500 has this same level of transparency but without much of the leaness. And therefore sounds even better. Sorry, I forgot that every CD player sounds different to you ![]() Not all, but many do. On the other hand, the fact that you raved about a $100 player means that it is possible to implement a good power supply in an inexpensive player, no? So you really can't say that the difference between "high-end" and "mid-fi" is in the power supply which shows up in "upper bass or lower midrange", since obviously you find the $100 Panasonic as good or better than some much more expensive players. Actually what I said was I suspected that the unit was fueled by powerDAC's. The leaness in the bass may very well be related to power supply. snip |
#113
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
MINe 109 wrote:
In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. In that case, you're on shaky ground ascribing the difference to "design," aren't you? I assume pianos are built to different designs without knowing how to build one. Fair enough. But I think it is significant that most of the participants here who are engineers seem to think that the design differences between CD players tend not to have audible consequences. I would trust their judgment over yours or mine. bob |
#114
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article , wrote:
MINe 109 wrote: In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: In article , wrote: MINe 109 wrote: I think my Arcam CD23 FMJ sounds better than my AMC CD8b, although I ascribe the difference to design, not to magic or "more money." Am I wrong? You might be. What part of the latter's design do you think is subpar, and how do you know that it is sufficiently subpar to be audible? I'd have to be a designer or engineer to make that kind of analysis. In that case, you're on shaky ground ascribing the difference to "design," aren't you? I assume pianos are built to different designs without knowing how to build one. Fair enough. But I think it is significant that most of the participants here who are engineers seem to think that the design differences between CD players tend not to have audible consequences. I would trust their judgment over yours or mine. I'd hate to be stuck with unsatisfactory gear because some engineer somewhere doesn't think audible consequences possible. I find it more reassuring when an engineer with a respectable audio track record points out things that can go wrong, like the pro-audio guy who found that cheap dvd players had clipped outputs due to poorly implemented DACs. One selling point of the Arcam is the RingDAC, which was sourced from dCS, who may be presumed to know something about design. Stephen |
#115
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
MINe 109 wrote:
Fair enough. But I think it is significant that most of the participants here who are engineers seem to think that the design differences between CD players tend not to have audible consequences. I would trust their judgment over yours or mine. I'd hate to be stuck with unsatisfactory gear because some engineer somewhere doesn't think audible consequences possible. I find it more reassuring when an engineer with a respectable audio track record points out things that can go wrong, like the pro-audio guy who found that cheap dvd players had clipped outputs due to poorly implemented DACs. That would show up clearly as THD (total harmonic distortion) in measurements. If you were to look at measurements of CD players, you will have a hard time finding any player with significant distortion, say above 0.05%. The DVD player you mentioned, if indeed your pro-audio guy was correct, is a very rare exception. One selling point of the Arcam is the RingDAC, which was sourced from dCS, who may be presumed to know something about design. Question, of course, is why would the other CD player(AMC) be noticeably worse in a listening test. Looking at the specs, there is nothing that indicates it would not be sonically accurate. Certainly the Burr-Brown 96/24 DAC's are very good performers. |
#116
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
MINe 109 wrote:
In article , wrote: Fair enough. But I think it is significant that most of the participants here who are engineers seem to think that the design differences between CD players tend not to have audible consequences. I would trust their judgment over yours or mine. I'd hate to be stuck with unsatisfactory gear because some engineer somewhere doesn't think audible consequences possible. I wouldn't take the word of one engineer either--unless the alternative was to take the word of a non-engineer! But every effect has a cause, and if you can't find any expert anywhere who can explain the cause, it's time to consider the possibility that you're misreading the effect. I find it more reassuring when an engineer with a respectable audio track record points out things that can go wrong, like the pro-audio guy who found that cheap dvd players had clipped outputs due to poorly implemented DACs. Missed that. Can you provide a reference? One selling point of the Arcam is the RingDAC, which was sourced from dCS, who may be presumed to know something about design. There are presumably many ways to design DACs. What's debatable is whether one way is enough better than another way to have audible consequences. bob |
#117
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
I assume pianos are built to different designs without knowing how to
build one. You picked an unfortunate example. Grand pianos reached the zenith of their design 100 years ago. Since that time, there have been almost no changes that could be fairly described as important. Indeed, a minor change in the way the strings are coupled to the bridge was the basis for an entirely new piano company in Australia. One could swap the entire work force of 2 different piano companies, and manufacturing would resume with scarcely a missed beat. Norm Strong |
#118
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
It is well known that, while 2V rms
is the 'standard' output for CD players, real values from player to player vary by up to 6dB. It is generally acknowledged that a 0.5dB difference will be heard by most listeners - but as a difference in *quality*, not loudness. I've heard 40 or 50 different CD players and couldn't - in blind testing conditions - identify one from the other. Therefore my belief has some substance to it. If there are players with boosted output levels, I have not heard them. The Eclipse deck I use in my car, which is also my CD transport, has 8 volt pre-amp outputs and is mated to a high end Zapco amp. The 8 volt pre-amp outputs are said to be cleaner than the 4 volt units used on MOST decks, although in all honesty, I don't think they make any audible difference. Turning up the gain on my amp makes an audible difference, but it has nothing to do with SOUND QUALITY it's just like volume boosting. If CD manufacturers are using some sort of gain to make their players louder, then it's a cheat, and although in those instances it will make a player sound DIFFERENT (maybe) it's not making the player sound BETTER. Furthur proof that there is NO audible difference between players unless something is WILLFULLY tampered with. |
#119
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article ,
wrote: I assume pianos are built to different designs without knowing how to build one. You picked an unfortunate example. Grand pianos reached the zenith of their design 100 years ago. Since that time, there have been almost no changes that could be fairly described as important. Indeed, a minor change in the way the strings are coupled to the bridge was the basis for an entirely new piano company in Australia. One could swap the entire work force of 2 different piano companies, and manufacturing would resume with scarcely a missed beat. 100 years ago one could buy an American Steinway (the "zenith" to which you refer), or a piano with a Viennese action, or a straight-strung Erard with under-dampers. Perhaps you are unaware of the piano-copying cottage industry. With enough money and enough lead time, one can purchase a copy or a restoration of a piano built in the 19th or 18th centuries. Unfortunate, indeed. Stephen |
#120
![]() |
|||
|
|||
![]()
In article , wrote:
MINe 109 wrote: In article , wrote: Fair enough. But I think it is significant that most of the participants here who are engineers seem to think that the design differences between CD players tend not to have audible consequences. I would trust their judgment over yours or mine. I'd hate to be stuck with unsatisfactory gear because some engineer somewhere doesn't think audible consequences possible. I wouldn't take the word of one engineer either--unless the alternative was to take the word of a non-engineer! But every effect has a cause, and if you can't find any expert anywhere who can explain the cause, it's time to consider the possibility that you're misreading the effect. "Trust me: I'm an EE," that kind of thing? Sounds like arguing from authority, especially if I'm told I'm not qualified to have an opinion. I find it more reassuring when an engineer with a respectable audio track record points out things that can go wrong, like the pro-audio guy who found that cheap dvd players had clipped outputs due to poorly implemented DACs. Missed that. Can you provide a reference? It was Ken Kantor on rec.audio.pro, about two years ago. One selling point of the Arcam is the RingDAC, which was sourced from dCS, who may be presumed to know something about design. There are presumably many ways to design DACs. What's debatable is whether one way is enough better than another way to have audible consequences. I doubt I could tell an Elgar from my CD23. Stephen |
Reply |
Thread Tools | |
Display Modes | |
|
|
![]() |
||||
Thread | Forum | |||
on topic: we need a rec.audio.pro.ot newsgroup! | Pro Audio | |||
DNC Schedule of Events | Pro Audio |