Reply
 
Thread Tools Display Modes
  #1   Report Post  
CaptLego
 
Posts: n/a
Default objecftivists --- what's the next step?

Consider the following hypothetical scenario:

I'm sitting at home listening to my stereo and reading an audio
magazine. I come across a review of some new cables. The review is
glowing with praise, and describes several attractive sonic
improvements. I'm thinking that my system could benefit from those
improvements. Maybe I go to the cable manufacturer's website and am
impressed by some pseudo-scientific mumbo jumbo about
perceptual-motion-electron-conditioning. I follow a link to a newage
site describing interactions of mother earth's energy fields and magic
cables.

I'm sold. I run down to the store and spend two month's salary on a
set of those cables. Returning home, I lovingly install them in my
system. I'm impressed with their build quality, and they look
fabulous. I turn on the system and do some critical listening. They
sound fantastic! The system really sounds better to me, and I'm
enjoying the music more than ever before.

Sounds better, and provides more satisfaction ---- that's a good
thing, right?
I'm so happy.

The next day, my objectivist friend comes to visit with some zipcord
and an ABX box. He proves to me that I can't tell the difference
between the two cables in a DBT test. He proves to me that the cables
are electrically equivalent. He proves to me that there is no sonic
difference between the two cables. He proves to me that the
differences I heard in my sighted test were caused by expectation
bias.

So what? What now? What next?

I really did perceive a difference in my sighted test, and I enjoyed
the magic cables much more than I enjoyed my previous cables.
Yesterday, my system sounded better (to me) after I instaled the new
cables. Today I understand that the difference I heard can be
attributed to non-sonic factors. That doesn't change the fact that
yesterday I perceived an improvement.

So what happens tomorrow?
1. Zipcord cables start sounding better? I now find the same
improvements were there all the time in zipcord? I return the
expensive new cables for a refund, but now get the same increased
enjoyment from zipcord? My newfound knowledge allows me to port my
perceptual improvements back to zipcord? Are there any studies that
indicate that the effects of expectation bias, once exposed, are
portable?

2. My new cables start sounding worse? The sound improvements I was
enjoying now vanish, and my system reverts to the sound it had two
days ago? My objectivist friend made my system sound worse to me?

3. My new cables still sound better to me? I know that the
improvement is not based on sonic factors, but they sound better
non-the-less. I understand that perception and judgement are based on
more than sound alone, and am willing to take advantage of that fact
if it increases my listening enjoyment.

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?
  #3   Report Post  
Chung
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:
Consider the following hypothetical scenario:

I'm sitting at home listening to my stereo and reading an audio
magazine. I come across a review of some new cables. The review is
glowing with praise, and describes several attractive sonic
improvements. I'm thinking that my system could benefit from those
improvements. Maybe I go to the cable manufacturer's website and am
impressed by some pseudo-scientific mumbo jumbo about
perceptual-motion-electron-conditioning. I follow a link to a newage
site describing interactions of mother earth's energy fields and magic
cables.

I'm sold. I run down to the store and spend two month's salary on a
set of those cables. Returning home, I lovingly install them in my
system. I'm impressed with their build quality, and they look
fabulous. I turn on the system and do some critical listening. They
sound fantastic! The system really sounds better to me, and I'm
enjoying the music more than ever before.

Sounds better, and provides more satisfaction ---- that's a good
thing, right?
I'm so happy.

The next day, my objectivist friend comes to visit with some zipcord
and an ABX box. He proves to me that I can't tell the difference
between the two cables in a DBT test. He proves to me that the cables
are electrically equivalent. He proves to me that there is no sonic
difference between the two cables. He proves to me that the
differences I heard in my sighted test were caused by expectation
bias.

So what? What now? What next?


Hopefully you now realize the power of expectation bias.


I really did perceive a difference in my sighted test, and I enjoyed
the magic cables much more than I enjoyed my previous cables.
Yesterday, my system sounded better (to me) after I instaled the new
cables. Today I understand that the difference I heard can be
attributed to non-sonic factors. That doesn't change the fact that
yesterday I perceived an improvement.

So what happens tomorrow?
1. Zipcord cables start sounding better? I now find the same
improvements were there all the time in zipcord? I return the
expensive new cables for a refund, but now get the same increased
enjoyment from zipcord? My newfound knowledge allows me to port my
perceptual improvements back to zipcord? Are there any studies that
indicate that the effects of expectation bias, once exposed, are
portable?


Of course, there is that not-so-small advantage of having an extra 2
months of salary...

I would guess that you have also learned that the expensive cables are a
rip-off. And that you never read another cable ad or review again
without a high degree of skepism.


2. My new cables start sounding worse? The sound improvements I was
enjoying now vanish, and my system reverts to the sound it had two
days ago? My objectivist friend made my system sound worse to me?


Well, if you want to believe that something sounds worse, you could
always convince yourself that it does. But why not simply stop worrying
about the cable after that illuminating experiment?


3. My new cables still sound better to me? I know that the
improvement is not based on sonic factors, but they sound better
non-the-less. I understand that perception and judgement are based on
more than sound alone, and am willing to take advantage of that fact
if it increases my listening enjoyment.


You of course are free to choose your gear whichever way you want to.


If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


A couple of things I can think of. You are helping to promote an
industry that is not based on solid engineering principles. And you
would be wrong if you try to convince others that the improvements come
from *sonic* factors.

Very few auidophiles are enlightened enough to realize that the
expensive cables they bought sound better to them because of purely
*non-sonic* factors, or that based on sonic factors, they perform as
well as zip-cord.
  #4   Report Post  
---MIKE---
 
Posts: n/a
Default

Here's what's wrong with that! Think of all the CDs or SACDs you could
buy with that two weeks salary you spent on the cables. I have noticed
that my system "sounds" different to me every day. Is it me or the
temperature, pressure, humidity, or other factors? The point is it does
sound different even though I have done nothing to cause this. The
biggest tweak of all is ------ TIME.


---MIKE---
  #6   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

"If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception of
audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that? Shouldn't my
quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the contribution of non-sonic
factors? If all competent audio electronics are essentially perfect, but
I can still attain dramatic improvements via non-sonic factors, what's
wrong with that?"

You began this by saying how, in this hypothetical, you were led to buy
them based on marketing and 3rd party reaction which could not be
cconfirmed. The question for yourself is how you control, or not, such in
your life. One common sequence for one in this situation is to quickly
wonder if there is not some next thing which produces better purely
psychic results and/or what majic in sound based on same are you missing;
which leads to imo a fool's errand; subject to the next better
marketing/publicity/publishing technique. In the meantime, displacing from
your budjet all those cds that psychic wire cost.
  #8   Report Post  
Billy Shears
 
Posts: n/a
Default

In article , (CaptLego)
wrote:

Consider the following hypothetical scenario:

I'm sitting at home listening to my stereo and reading an audio
magazine. I come across a review of some new cables. The review is
glowing with praise, and describes several attractive sonic
improvements. I'm thinking that my system could benefit from those
improvements. Maybe I go to the cable manufacturer's website and am
impressed by some pseudo-scientific mumbo jumbo about
perceptual-motion-electron-conditioning. I follow a link to a newage
site describing interactions of mother earth's energy fields and magic
cables.

I'm sold. I run down to the store and spend two month's salary on a
set of those cables. Returning home, I lovingly install them in my
system. I'm impressed with their build quality, and they look
fabulous. I turn on the system and do some critical listening. They
sound fantastic! The system really sounds better to me, and I'm
enjoying the music more than ever before.

Sounds better, and provides more satisfaction ---- that's a good
thing, right?
I'm so happy.

The next day, my objectivist friend comes to visit with some zipcord
and an ABX box. He proves to me that I can't tell the difference
between the two cables in a DBT test. He proves to me that the cables
are electrically equivalent. He proves to me that there is no sonic
difference between the two cables. He proves to me that the
differences I heard in my sighted test were caused by expectation
bias.

So what? What now? What next?

I really did perceive a difference in my sighted test, and I enjoyed
the magic cables much more than I enjoyed my previous cables.
Yesterday, my system sounded better (to me) after I instaled the new
cables. Today I understand that the difference I heard can be
attributed to non-sonic factors. That doesn't change the fact that
yesterday I perceived an improvement.

So what happens tomorrow?
1. Zipcord cables start sounding better? I now find the same
improvements were there all the time in zipcord? I return the
expensive new cables for a refund, but now get the same increased
enjoyment from zipcord? My newfound knowledge allows me to port my
perceptual improvements back to zipcord? Are there any studies that
indicate that the effects of expectation bias, once exposed, are
portable?

2. My new cables start sounding worse? The sound improvements I was
enjoying now vanish, and my system reverts to the sound it had two
days ago? My objectivist friend made my system sound worse to me?

3. My new cables still sound better to me? I know that the
improvement is not based on sonic factors, but they sound better
non-the-less. I understand that perception and judgement are based on
more than sound alone, and am willing to take advantage of that fact
if it increases my listening enjoyment.

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


You've learned that non-sonic factors can enhance your enjoyment of audio.
Santa Claus can enhance the enjoyment of Christmas for a child. But the
child needs to learn it was all a hoax at some point, as do you. It's a big
letdown at first. But the child will learn to cope and so will you. In my
opinion we shouldn't be lying to our children about Santa Claus to begin
with. And the audio sales guy shouldn't be lying about quantum mechanical
cleansing of electrons in the speaker cables he's trying to foist on you.
But lies happen. When you discover them, it's time to return the $4,000
cable. The improvement you heard was your enthusiasm for audio being
rathceted up by your own psychology. The great news is you will undoubtedly
hear improvement again, but next time it will occur in the non Santa Claus
realm. Merry Christmas.

--The Grinch
  #10   Report Post  
Chung
 
Posts: n/a
Default

normanstrong wrote:
"J=F3n Fairbairn" wrote in message
...
(CaptLego) writes:


If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception
[...] -- then why not take advantage of that?


One reason for the objectivist's ire is that it would not be
you that is taking the advantage, but the seller of the
"magic" wires or whatever.

=20
My wife likes Starbucks french roast coffee, and has said that I
should buy it because we can afford it, and she feels that she should
be able to splurge a bit at this time in our lives. However, Costco
also carries another brand of french roast that costs half as much as
Starbucks. I cannot tell any difference in flavor between the two;
neither can my wife. But, if she knows that I've substituted the
cheaper beans for the Starbucks, she's incensed. It's come to the
point where she feels she can't be sure that she's drinking Starbucks
unless she makes the coffee herself. Yesterday, she even brought up
the possibility that I might possibly have filled an empty Starbucks
bag with the cheaper beans without telling her. The entire situation
is fantastic to me--indeed, quite amusing. But to my wife it's a
matter of trust, love, and a willingness to spend to make her happy.
After all, "she's worth it."
=20
I think something similar to this goes on in the audiophile world.
The knowledge of what you're listening to has more effect than the
actual sound.
=20
Norm Strong


FWIW, Costco also sells pre-packaged Starbucks brand coffee. I believe=20
thr French-roast goes for about $9 for 2 pounds, about half what it=20
costs at Starbucks.

I personally prefer Peetz's coffee, and they usually give you a free=20
fresh-brewed cup if you buy a pound.

But to back up your point, I know people who got angry when they found=20
out that their expensive cables sound the same as zip-cords, or that=20
their magical discs have no effects. Unfortunately they also got angry=20
at the messengers for making them look stupid...


  #11   Report Post  
Billy Shears
 
Posts: n/a
Default

In article ,
"normanstrong" wrote:

"Jón Fairbairn" wrote in message
...
(CaptLego) writes:


If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception
[...] -- then why not take advantage of that?


One reason for the objectivist's ire is that it would not be
you that is taking the advantage, but the seller of the
"magic" wires or whatever.


My wife likes Starbucks french roast coffee, and has said that I
should buy it because we can afford it, and she feels that she should
be able to splurge a bit at this time in our lives. However, Costco
also carries another brand of french roast that costs half as much as
Starbucks. I cannot tell any difference in flavor between the two;
neither can my wife. But, if she knows that I've substituted the
cheaper beans for the Starbucks, she's incensed. It's come to the
point where she feels she can't be sure that she's drinking Starbucks
unless she makes the coffee herself. Yesterday, she even brought up
the possibility that I might possibly have filled an empty Starbucks
bag with the cheaper beans without telling her. The entire situation
is fantastic to me--indeed, quite amusing. But to my wife it's a
matter of trust, love, and a willingness to spend to make her happy.
After all, "she's worth it."

I think something similar to this goes on in the audiophile world.
The knowledge of what you're listening to has more effect than the
actual sound.


Well no, because the tweakophile doesn't acknowledge there is no
difference. How much megabucks speaker cable is sold to customers who know
it sounds the same as zip cord? In fact, the tweakophile, while probably
deluded, makes more sense here than does your wife, although I'm sure she's
a lovely woman.
  #12   Report Post  
Greg Grainger
 
Posts: n/a
Default

In article , CaptLego wrote:
Consider the following hypothetical scenario:


[ . . . ]

The next day, my objectivist friend comes to visit with some zipcord
and an ABX box. He proves to me that I can't tell the difference
between the two cables in a DBT test. He proves to me that the cables
are electrically equivalent. He proves to me that there is no sonic
difference between the two cables. He proves to me that the
differences I heard in my sighted test were caused by expectation
bias.


[ . . . ]

There's a segment of 'Zen and the Art of Motorcycle Maintenance' where the
hero's friend comes to him with a problem with his (very expensive) German
motorcycle. The hero identifies the problem, and tells the friend that the
solution requires the insertion of a metal shim. The friend wonders how
much this will cost. The hero says, 'nothing' and indicates that perfectly
good shims can be made out of an old beer can hammered flat and cut to
shape.

The friend is horrified and makes many excuses, eventually taking his
expensive motorcycle to a factory mechanic, where he was undoubtedly
mercilessly overcharged for 'factory spec' shims of the same functionality
as the beer can.

The same sort of thing can be found with camera buffs who buy only the
best Nikon or Zeiss lenses when a Vivitar or Soligor will do just as well,
at a fraction of the cost.

I wonder if there isn't some little part of our brains that tells us that
putting zip cord in place of expensive speaker cables isn't a form of
pollution, and that, sound quality aside, we have degraded our 'noble'
systems with 'lower-class' components.

Are we really interested in sound quality, or in impressing our friends
and reassuring ourselves that we have done everything in our power to get
the purest sound possible. That 'purity,' of course, must include the
pedigree of the components which produce the sound, otherwise there can be
no justice or beauty in the world.

I, for one, will be going to the electrical supply store and investing in
some 10-gauge, 2-wire, SJ or SO rated 'cab-tire.' Like zip cord, but with
two or three times the cross-section. More expensive than zip cord, but
still vastly cheaper than some of the audio cables advertised.

Then we'll see.

Best,
Greg.
--
Greg Grainger

'What a world of gammon and spinach it is, though, ain't it?'
- Miss Mowcher
  #13   Report Post  
Nousaine
 
Posts: n/a
Default

Stewart Pinkerton wrote:
On 4 Dec 2004 17:03:12 GMT,
(CaptLego) wrote:

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


That seems pretty obvious. Use functional electronics, and sell your
granny and your kids to buy Avalon Eidolons (or substitute the
aspirational speaker of your choice).
--

Stewart Pinkerton | Music is Art - Audio is Engineering


Here's another anaysis I made nearly 10 years ago after conducting a controlled
listening tests of Tara Labs RSC cables costing $990 a pair compared to a set
of zipcord wires. The cables were not reliably distinguishable to a 21-year old
female (and my self) so I compiled an economic and aesthetic comparison of the
things a $1000 bought during the first 3 months of that calendar year.

Option 1:

RSC Cables: $990
1 CD: $6 (Chas. Neville)

Total: $996

Option 2:

Equipment/Software

Zip Cord $5
Pamona Banana Plugs $8
2 CDs $26
2 Laser Discs $95

Education:

AES Section Meeting: $15 (dinner)

Live Music: (prices for 2 tix)

CSO $132
Neville Bros Concert: $70
DrJohn Concert $165 (includes lodging)
Leon Russell Concert $134 ( " )
Decatur Jazz Festival $265 ( " )
Chicago Jazz Fair $30

Total $945

I think the latter course, which reflects what I actually did and not what I
might have otherwise done musically, is a much more useful course of action
....after all, the sound at home was exactly the same either way.
  #14   Report Post  
Georg Grosz
 
Posts: n/a
Default

(CaptLego) wrote in message ...

... If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


I suppose in principle, there should be nothing wrong with non-sonic
improvements, just so long as sensibly priced and rationally designed
gear remains available. Causes for optimism:

* Lamps continue to run on electricity, guaranteeing a long term
supply of lamp cord for our speakers.

* Panasonic and Sony are showing no signs of going out of business.

* Recording media will be increasingly driven by the computer
industry.

But I disagree with your statement. All competent audio electronics
are not essentially perfect. There are many areas of potential
improvement:

* Ergonomics and miniaturization

* Safety and reliability

* Cost reduction with no loss in audio quality

* Ways to make speakers work better in non-ideal acoustic
environments?

Hopefully, audiophile marketing does not distract the industry from
making the objectively desirable improvements listed above. But again,
there is no cause for pessimism. Good engineering talent is not wasted
on developing psychic audiophile products, because no engineering
talent is involved.

For better or worse, most of the audiophile stuff is out of my price
range. I am as likely to buy $100 cables as I am to buy $10000 cables.
Of potential interest is a new sound reinforcement system made by
Bose, which appears to be capable of reasonable amplification without
feedback problems -- addressing the fourth issue listed above. But to
be honest, I am saving my pennies to improve my sound at the source,
by buying a new string bass.

To summarize my view, there's lots of cool things yet to be
discovered, but not by the audiophiles. Their world does not intersect
with mine. To borrow from Thomas Jefferson: It matters not whether my
neighbor believes in no audiophile tweeks or fifty -- it neither
breaks my leg nor picks my pocket.
  #15   Report Post  
Bob Ross
 
Posts: n/a
Default

Chung wrote in message


I know people who got angry when they found
out that their expensive cables sound the same as zip-cords, or that
their magical discs have no effects.



My expensive cables sounded worse than zip cord. Seriously.


  #16   Report Post  
CaptLego
 
Posts: n/a
Default

Chung wrote in message ...
CaptLego wrote:
Consider the following hypothetical scenario:
[snip]


Hopefully you now realize the power of expectation bias.


Powerful stuff, indeed. It seems too powerful to ignore, or to simply
wish it would go away. I suppose it could be used for good (improving
the sound of a system), or evil (fleecing ignorant consumers).

[more stuff snipped]

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


A couple of things I can think of. You are helping to promote an
industry that is not based on solid engineering principles. And you
would be wrong if you try to convince others that the improvements come
from *sonic* factors.


Agreed. But is it too ambitious to think that it could be put on a
solid engineering basis? Does it depend upon how much a gizmo costs?
On the credibility of proponents? Appearance? Could economical audio
systems be improved in this regard, or is this powerful effect only
something to be exploited by snakeoil salesmen?


Very few auidophiles are enlightened enough to realize that the
expensive cables they bought sound better to them because of purely
*non-sonic* factors, or that based on sonic factors, they perform as
well as zip-cord.


You're probably right.

But if you'd indulge me in a bit more of my hypothetical story.....

The other night my objectivist friend came over for dinner. Now I
like to eat peas, and I was excited to serve a new brand of peas that
I'd recently discovered. My old peas were OK, I guess, but they were
kinda grey and smelled funny. The new peas are a nice green, and
smell good, too. I think they taste better. My objectivist friend
laughed at me, and told me I'd been duped again. He explained that
all peas taste the same (given that they are the same size and
texture, and that less than 1% are spoiled). To prove it to me, he
blindfolded me and put a clothspin on my nose. Then he fed me a
serving of each brand of peas. Sure enough, I could not tell any
difference based on taste alone. Once again, I was influenced by
"expectation bias".

Or was I? I don't normally eat dinner blindfolded with a clothspin on
my nose. Maybe in normal dining circumstances, my olfactory inputs
get combined with the signals from my tastebuds in some
incomprehensible neurological process of my brain. Perhaps I
(mistakenly?) attribute the change to "taste". Or maybe the sight of
green peas on my plate somehow recalibrates my tastebuds in the
expectation of a different sensation. This is, of course, pure
speculation on my part. But I know that I still prefer to eat the
green peas. My friend thinks I'm crazy. I guess there's just no
accounting for taste....

I expect that in the food industry, they study the interaction of
appearance, smell, and taste. I don't know if there's sensory
co-mingling in the audio realm, but *something* is going on that makes
reviewers hear dramatic differences. There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.

Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?
  #18   Report Post  
S888Wheel
 
Posts: n/a
Default

From: (Greg Grainger)
Date: 12/5/2004 4:29 PM Pacific Standard Time
Message-id:

In article , CaptLego
wrote:
Consider the following hypothetical scenario:


[ . . . ]

The next day, my objectivist friend comes to visit with some zipcord
and an ABX box. He proves to me that I can't tell the difference
between the two cables in a DBT test. He proves to me that the cables
are electrically equivalent. He proves to me that there is no sonic
difference between the two cables. He proves to me that the
differences I heard in my sighted test were caused by expectation
bias.


[ . . . ]

There's a segment of 'Zen and the Art of Motorcycle Maintenance' where the
hero's friend comes to him with a problem with his (very expensive) German
motorcycle. The hero identifies the problem, and tells the friend that the
solution requires the insertion of a metal shim. The friend wonders how
much this will cost. The hero says, 'nothing' and indicates that perfectly
good shims can be made out of an old beer can hammered flat and cut to
shape.

The friend is horrified and makes many excuses, eventually taking his
expensive motorcycle to a factory mechanic, where he was undoubtedly
mercilessly overcharged for 'factory spec' shims of the same functionality
as the beer can.

The same sort of thing can be found with camera buffs who buy only the
best Nikon or Zeiss lenses when a Vivitar or Soligor will do just as well,
at a fraction of the cost.


Actually they won't. whether the difference matters to you or not is another
issue but there is a difference. Same is true with coffee for that matter.


I wonder if there isn't some little part of our brains that tells us that
putting zip cord in place of expensive speaker cables isn't a form of
pollution, and that, sound quality aside, we have degraded our 'noble'
systems with 'lower-class' components.

Are we really interested in sound quality, or in impressing our friends


For me it is sound quality.


and reassuring ourselves that we have done everything in our power to get
the purest sound possible.


Now this seems like a legitimate question. Do people do things just to be sure
they have done all they can? I think that may come into play at times. The old
saying, better safe than sorry. I can see that as an influence.

That 'purity,' of course, must include the
pedigree of the components which produce the sound, otherwise there can be
no justice or beauty in the world.


Now I think you are just going over the top.



I, for one, will be going to the electrical supply store and investing in
some 10-gauge, 2-wire, SJ or SO rated 'cab-tire.' Like zip cord, but with
two or three times the cross-section. More expensive than zip cord, but
still vastly cheaper than some of the audio cables advertised.


Why? If zip chord is just as good?



Then we'll see.


I doubt it.



  #19   Report Post  
Nousaine
 
Posts: n/a
Default

(Georg Grosz) wrote:



(CaptLego) wrote in message
...

... If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


I suppose in principle, there should be nothing wrong with non-sonic
improvements, just so long as sensibly priced and rationally designed
gear remains available. Causes for optimism:

* Lamps continue to run on electricity, guaranteeing a long term
supply of lamp cord for our speakers.

* Panasonic and Sony are showing no signs of going out of business.

* Recording media will be increasingly driven by the computer
industry.

But I disagree with your statement. All competent audio electronics
are not essentially perfect. There are many areas of potential
improvement:

* Ergonomics and miniaturization

* Safety and reliability

* Cost reduction with no loss in audio quality

* Ways to make speakers work better in non-ideal acoustic
environments?

Hopefully, audiophile marketing does not distract the industry from
making the objectively desirable improvements listed above. But again,
there is no cause for pessimism. Good engineering talent is not wasted
on developing psychic audiophile products, because no engineering
talent is involved.

For better or worse, most of the audiophile stuff is out of my price
range. I am as likely to buy $100 cables as I am to buy $10000 cables.
Of potential interest is a new sound reinforcement system made by
Bose, which appears to be capable of reasonable amplification without
feedback problems -- addressing the fourth issue listed above. But to
be honest, I am saving my pennies to improve my sound at the source,
by buying a new string bass.

To summarize my view, there's lots of cool things yet to be
discovered, but not by the audiophiles. Their world does not intersect
with mine. To borrow from Thomas Jefferson: It matters not whether my
neighbor believes in no audiophile tweeks or fifty -- it neither
breaks my leg nor picks my pocket.


I think that 2005 will be the start of a new and wonderful trend in
loudspeakers that will include outboard and on-board speaker dsp control
including auto-eq and auto-room compensation.
  #21   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:

Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?


In other words, should Stereophile review the marketing gibberish
rather than the cables themselves? Let me think how this would go:

"The brochure for the Quantum 9000 cables was packaged in a heavy paper
envelope, with excellent build quality. When I read it aloud, even my
wife could hear it from the kitchen."
  #22   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:
Chung wrote in message

...
CaptLego wrote:


snip

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and

my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate*

perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio

electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic

improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


A couple of things I can think of. You are helping to promote an
industry that is not based on solid engineering principles. And you


would be wrong if you try to convince others that the improvements

come
from *sonic* factors.


Agreed. But is it too ambitious to think that it could be put on a
solid engineering basis? Does it depend upon how much a gizmo costs?


On the credibility of proponents? Appearance? Could economical

audio
systems be improved in this regard, or is this powerful effect only
something to be exploited by snakeoil salesmen?


You are completely missing the point. You aren't "improving" an audio
system by imagining that it is better than it is. You may be improving
your enjoyment of it, but if you want to believe that something is
better, you don't need to spend megabucks to do it. Consider the
immense "improvement" reported by such cheap tweaks as green pens. Any
change has the potential to invoke a positive mental response.

Very few auidophiles are enlightened enough to realize that the
expensive cables they bought sound better to them because of purely


*non-sonic* factors, or that based on sonic factors, they perform

as
well as zip-cord.


You're probably right.

But if you'd indulge me in a bit more of my hypothetical story.....

The other night my objectivist friend came over for dinner. Now I
like to eat peas, and I was excited to serve a new brand of peas that
I'd recently discovered. My old peas were OK, I guess, but they were
kinda grey and smelled funny. The new peas are a nice green, and
smell good, too. I think they taste better. My objectivist friend
laughed at me, and told me I'd been duped again. He explained that
all peas taste the same (given that they are the same size and
texture, and that less than 1% are spoiled). To prove it to me, he
blindfolded me and put a clothspin on my nose.


Why a clothespin? You've rigged the test right there, since most of
what we "taste" is actually smelled. Do the test with just a blindfold,
and it's a fair analogy. (And fresh peas will win eaasily.)

Then he fed me a
serving of each brand of peas. Sure enough, I could not tell any
difference based on taste alone. Once again, I was influenced by
"expectation bias".

Or was I? I don't normally eat dinner blindfolded with a clothspin

on
my nose. Maybe in normal dining circumstances, my olfactory inputs
get combined with the signals from my tastebuds in some
incomprehensible neurological process of my brain. Perhaps I
(mistakenly?) attribute the change to "taste". Or maybe the sight of
green peas on my plate somehow recalibrates my tastebuds in the
expectation of a different sensation. This is, of course, pure
speculation on my part. But I know that I still prefer to eat the
green peas. My friend thinks I'm crazy. I guess there's just no
accounting for taste....

I expect that in the food industry, they study the interaction of
appearance, smell, and taste. I don't know if there's sensory
co-mingling in the audio realm, but *something* is going on that

makes
reviewers hear dramatic differences. There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.

Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?


Who's bemoaning it? We're just accepting it, and learning to account
for it in our comparisons. And by the way, what is it that you think we
don't understand about it?

bob
  #23   Report Post  
Chung
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:
Chung wrote in message ...
CaptLego wrote:
Consider the following hypothetical scenario:
[snip]


Hopefully you now realize the power of expectation bias.


Powerful stuff, indeed. It seems too powerful to ignore, or to simply
wish it would go away.


When it comes to speaker cables, green pens, etc., a lot of people
believe that they can overcome the expectation bias.

I suppose it could be used for good (improving
the sound of a system), or evil (fleecing ignorant consumers).


And what is good for those who sell such cables may not be good for you.


[more stuff snipped]

If we know that non-sonic factors can influence perception (and my
objectivist friend argues that non-sonic factors *dominate* perception
of audio electronics) -- then why not take advantage of that?
Shouldn't my quest for audio nirvana focus on maximizing the
contribution of non-sonic factors? If all competent audio electronics
are essentially perfect, but I can still attain dramatic improvements
via non-sonic factors, what's wrong with that?


A couple of things I can think of. You are helping to promote an
industry that is not based on solid engineering principles. And you
would be wrong if you try to convince others that the improvements come
from *sonic* factors.


Agreed. But is it too ambitious to think that it could be put on a
solid engineering basis?


Put what on a solid engineering basis?

Does it depend upon how much a gizmo costs?
On the credibility of proponents? Appearance? Could economical audio
systems be improved in this regard, or is this powerful effect only
something to be exploited by snakeoil salesmen?


Huh?



Very few auidophiles are enlightened enough to realize that the
expensive cables they bought sound better to them because of purely
*non-sonic* factors, or that based on sonic factors, they perform as
well as zip-cord.


You're probably right.

But if you'd indulge me in a bit more of my hypothetical story.....

The other night my objectivist friend came over for dinner. Now I
like to eat peas, and I was excited to serve a new brand of peas that
I'd recently discovered. My old peas were OK, I guess, but they were
kinda grey and smelled funny. The new peas are a nice green, and
smell good, too. I think they taste better. My objectivist friend
laughed at me, and told me I'd been duped again. He explained that
all peas taste the same (given that they are the same size and
texture, and that less than 1% are spoiled). To prove it to me, he
blindfolded me and put a clothspin on my nose. Then he fed me a
serving of each brand of peas. Sure enough, I could not tell any
difference based on taste alone. Once again, I was influenced by
"expectation bias".

Or was I? I don't normally eat dinner blindfolded with a clothspin on
my nose. Maybe in normal dining circumstances, my olfactory inputs
get combined with the signals from my tastebuds in some
incomprehensible neurological process of my brain. Perhaps I
(mistakenly?) attribute the change to "taste". Or maybe the sight of
green peas on my plate somehow recalibrates my tastebuds in the
expectation of a different sensation. This is, of course, pure
speculation on my part. But I know that I still prefer to eat the
green peas. My friend thinks I'm crazy. I guess there's just no
accounting for taste....


When it comes to food, sight and smell are very important sensations.
I'm not sure what you proved.

There is nothing wrong with preferring food that looks better and smells
better. Those are important criteria for your preference.


I expect that in the food industry, they study the interaction of
appearance, smell, and taste. I don't know if there's sensory
co-mingling in the audio realm, but *something* is going on that makes
reviewers hear dramatic differences.


That something is "expection bias" when it comes to green pens, shatki
stones and mpingo disks. Certainly there can be dramatic differences
between some components, but in every one of those instances the
differencs can be clearly supported by measurements.

There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.


You dim the lights, and your system would probably sound different. Or
you can have a glass of wine and it will sound different. Your state of
mind has a large effect.


Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?


Not sure who is bemoaning it. Certainly not those you call objectivists.
Perhaps those who bought the cables and found out that they sound the
same as zip-cords?

Scientists have observed this phenomenen for years. It's just basic
psychology.
  #24   Report Post  
Bob Ross
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:


Maybe in normal dining circumstances, my olfactory inputs
get combined with the signals from my tastebuds in some
incomprehensible neurological process of my brain.


"Maybe"? "MAYBE"??? You'd better believe they're inextricably
linked...and anyone in the olfactory sciences is well aware of this
connection.
What you describe isn't a Double Blind Test, it's a Double Dumb-Ass
Test!


I don't know if there's sensory
co-mingling in the audio realm, but *something* is going on that

makes
reviewers hear dramatic differences. There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.


Right. That's the Expectation Bias that everyone is talking about.


Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?


In order to study it, one first has to isolate it. And not confuse it
with other factors. (Such as actual sonic differences between audio
components.) Likewise, when one studies actual sonic differences
between audio components, one has to isolate them, and not confuse them
with other factors such as Expectation Bias. That's just the nature of
good science.
  #26   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

wrote:
CaptLego wrote:
Chung wrote in message

...
CaptLego wrote:


[snip]

Agreed. But is it too ambitious to think that it could be put on a
solid engineering basis? Does it depend upon how much a gizmo

costs?

On the credibility of proponents? Appearance? Could economical

audio
systems be improved in this regard, or is this powerful effect only
something to be exploited by snakeoil salesmen?


You are completely missing the point. You aren't "improving" an audio
system by imagining that it is better than it is. You may be

improving
your enjoyment of it, but if you want to believe that something is
better, you don't need to spend megabucks to do it. Consider the
immense "improvement" reported by such cheap tweaks as green pens.

Any
change has the potential to invoke a positive mental response.


I think my point is that subjective reviewers cannot distinguish
between imagined and real improvements. For some components
(electronics, for example), a DBT or other objective measurements may
be able to sort it out. But if normal listening conditions are
sighted, then an imagined improvement may be every bit as "real" and
effective as a sonic one. Hence, the need to understand it --- how
stable is it? Does it depend on cost, appearance, scientific mumbo
jumbo, reviews, or what? How do I eliminate (or exploit) it when
making accoustical treatments of my room?

In audio, the ultimate end of the chain is some mysterious perception
process in the human brain --- not the acoustic waves in a room. The
ultimate measure is an intellectual or emotional response in a human.
That's a more complex process than just putting accoustical waves into
the air.



[snip]

Why a clothespin? You've rigged the test right there, since most of
what we "taste" is actually smelled. Do the test with just a

blindfold,
and it's a fair analogy. (And fresh peas will win eaasily.)

[snip]
And apparently, a lot of what we hear is imagined. That's why bizarre
tweaks can sound better, I guess. If it's fair to remove the clothspin
from my nose when doing taste tests, why is it not fair to remove the
blindfold when doing listening tests? For that matter, why is it fair
to remove the clothspin, but not the blindfold? If smell can influence
taste, why can't appearance?


... reviewers hear dramatic differences. There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.

Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it, rather
than bemoan its existence?


Who's bemoaning it? We're just accepting it, and learning to account
for it in our comparisons. And by the way, what is it that you think

we
don't understand about it?


I think you're mostly concerned with eliminating it. The argument goes
like this:
a) Magic cables sound better only because of expectation bias.
b) Zipcord is sonically equilivant to magic cables
c) Therefore, people should use zipcord, saving the $$ for more
effective or useful things.

At first, that seems quite logical. But that leaves us with the
conclusion that something which improves the sound (in the mind of the
listener -- which is the only thing that matters) should be eliminated.
Somehow, the physical aspects of audio are legimitate, but the
perceptual processes involved are not?

A few years ago, AI (Artificial Intelligence) research was hot. A
colleague of mine said "It's only artificial intelligence when you
don't understand it. Once you understand it, it's just an algorithm.)

Magic cables are only "magic" if we don't understand how they work. If
they work by affecting the perceptual process in the brain, is that any
less legitimate than by affecting the acoustics in the room?

Lossy encoding of audio and video (e.g. MP3) is effective because it
exploits perceptual encoding --- if you're going to compromise the
objective fidelity, do it in the way that is the least noticable to
humans. If non-sonic factors can dramatically (and positively) affect
the perception of audio, we should find the most cost-effective way to
stimulate them. Maybe the best way to further improve the "sound" of a
system is spend money on lighting, or a HDTV with some relaxing images,
or the right kind of drugs. I do find that I enjoy symphonic music
much more when I can see the orchestra...

Are pshycoacoustic phenomena outside the scope of recreational audio
--- by definition?





bob

  #27   Report Post  
Steven Sullivan
 
Posts: n/a
Default

CaptLego wrote:
Chung wrote in message ...
The other night my objectivist friend came over for dinner. Now I
like to eat peas, and I was excited to serve a new brand of peas that
I'd recently discovered. My old peas were OK, I guess, but they were
kinda grey and smelled funny. The new peas are a nice green, and
smell good, too. I think they taste better. My objectivist friend
laughed at me, and told me I'd been duped again. He explained that
all peas taste the same (given that they are the same size and
texture, and that less than 1% are spoiled). To prove it to me, he
blindfolded me and put a clothspin on my nose. Then he fed me a
serving of each brand of peas. Sure enough, I could not tell any
difference based on taste alone. Once again, I was influenced by
"expectation bias".


Or was I? I don't normally eat dinner blindfolded with a clothspin on
my nose. Maybe in normal dining circumstances, my olfactory inputs
get combined with the signals from my tastebuds in some
incomprehensible neurological process of my brain.


In fact, they are. It's not incomprehensible; the neural tracts
connecting olfactory to taste centers were mapped long ago, I think.
It's why when you have a stuffy nose, food doesn't taste as
distinctive.

And you really shouldn't set up a test with so many variables
changed at once. Moreover, while olfactory input certainly
impairs perception of gustatory difference, there';s no
evidence AFAIK that literally blinding someone does sthe same
to auditory difference perception (indeed, some people probably
feel they can concentrate *better* with eyes closed)

But experimental 'blinding' isnt' the same as blindfolding, anyway.
Supposem instead, that your friend had simply given you two
plates of peas in succession, without 'blindfolding' you, and varied whether
the two plates were the same or different? What sensory
input are you missing?

DBTs aren't actually done 'blindfolded'. It's quite possible
to set them up so that the devices under test are all *visible*.
All that's needed is a way to mask which one is connected to the
system during the test. 'Blinding' in a DBT refers to
keeping biasing *knowledge* from the subject, not necessarily
visual input.



--
-S
Your a boring little troll. How does it feel? Go blow your bad breath elsewhere.
  #28   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

wrote:

I think my point is that subjective reviewers cannot distinguish
between imagined and real improvements. For some components
(electronics, for example), a DBT or other objective measurements may
be able to sort it out. But if normal listening conditions are
sighted, then an imagined improvement may be every bit as "real" and
effective as a sonic one. Hence, the need to understand it --- how
stable is it?


Quite. Audiophiles seem to hang onto those impressions like snapping
turtles.

Does it depend on cost, appearance, scientific mumbo
jumbo, reviews, or what?


Any or all of the above, and it varies by individual, and for each
individual with the phases of the moon.

How do I eliminate (or exploit) it when
making accoustical treatments of my room?


You can't, as a practical matter. So in that case, you just have to try
to listen as critically as possible. But you can be reasonably certain
that any well-designed acoustical treatment (i.e., not that silly
curved wood thingy) _will_ have an audible effect.

In audio, the ultimate end of the chain is some mysterious perception
process in the human brain --- not the acoustic waves in a room. The
ultimate measure is an intellectual or emotional response in a human.
That's a more complex process than just putting accoustical waves

into
the air.



[snip]

Why a clothespin? You've rigged the test right there, since most of
what we "taste" is actually smelled. Do the test with just a

blindfold,
and it's a fair analogy. (And fresh peas will win eaasily.)

[snip]
And apparently, a lot of what we hear is imagined. That's why

bizarre
tweaks can sound better, I guess. If it's fair to remove the

clothspin
from my nose when doing taste tests, why is it not fair to remove the
blindfold when doing listening tests? For that matter, why is it

fair
to remove the clothspin, but not the blindfold? If smell can

influence
taste, why can't appearance?


It all depends on what you want to test. If you want to test the
sensitivity of your tongue, then you need the clothespin. Whereas, if
you want to find out if Norm's wife is a sucker for Starbucks
marketing, just don't tell her which coffee is in which cup (which is
what "blind" means here, BTW).


... reviewers hear dramatic differences. There is apparently some
confounding factor that has a major influence on the perception

of
audio -- beyond the sonic performance alone.

Such a powerful effect --- why not study and understand it,

rather
than bemoan its existence?


Who's bemoaning it? We're just accepting it, and learning to

account
for it in our comparisons. And by the way, what is it that you

think
we
don't understand about it?


I think you're mostly concerned with eliminating it. The argument

goes
like this:
a) Magic cables sound better only because of expectation bias.
b) Zipcord is sonically equilivant to magic cables
c) Therefore, people should use zipcord, saving the $$ for more
effective or useful things.

At first, that seems quite logical. But that leaves us with the
conclusion that something which improves the sound (in the mind of

the
listener -- which is the only thing that matters) should be

eliminated.
Somehow, the physical aspects of audio are legimitate, but the
perceptual processes involved are not?


You're putting words in my mouth. I'm not demanding that anyone compare
components in a particular way. If buying megabuck cables makes you
happy, go for it.

A few years ago, AI (Artificial Intelligence) research was hot. A
colleague of mine said "It's only artificial intelligence when you
don't understand it. Once you understand it, it's just an

algorithm.)

Magic cables are only "magic" if we don't understand how they work.

If
they work by affecting the perceptual process in the brain, is that

any
less legitimate than by affecting the acoustics in the room?


Who called anything illegitimate? You're putting words in my mouth
again.

Lossy encoding of audio and video (e.g. MP3) is effective because it
exploits perceptual encoding --- if you're going to compromise the
objective fidelity, do it in the way that is the least noticable to
humans. If non-sonic factors can dramatically (and positively)

affect
the perception of audio, we should find the most cost-effective way

to
stimulate them. Maybe the best way to further improve the "sound" of

a
system is spend money on lighting, or a HDTV with some relaxing

images,
or the right kind of drugs. I do find that I enjoy symphonic music
much more when I can see the orchestra...

Are pshycoacoustic phenomena outside the scope of recreational audio
--- by definition?


Of course not. *Hearing* is a psychoacoustic phenomenon. I think what
you mean is, why can't we take advantage of the placebo effect, if it
brings us joy? And my answer is, go right ahead. Just don't then come
back and tell me that it's something else at work.

bob
  #30   Report Post  
---MIKE---
 
Posts: n/a
Default

Have you considered this? Atmospheric pressure, temperature and
humidity varies from day to day (we had a violent wind storm last night
which was a result of a dramatic drop in pressure). These elements have
an effect on the speed of sound. Since the room dimensions remain
constant, a change in the speed of sound would cause the frequency of
room nodes to change. Wouldn't this cause the apparent sound of a
stereo system to change?


---MIKE---


  #32   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

First, I'd like to thank those who have responded (except for the guy
that called me a troll).

But so far, nobody has attempted to answer my original question.
I'm trying to understand expectation bias. I'm trying to find out how
well it is understood. I'm curious how much progress has been made in
really understanding it, and if it can (has?) been characterized
sufficiently for engineering purposes.

To me, that means that we have sufficient analytical skills and
knowledge to predict the outcome of experiments that involve
expectation bias. For example, I was wondering how robust expectation
bias is, and in particular how effective DBT experience might be in
changing expectation bias.

So I ask for predictions of the outcome of the following experiment.

1. Start with a group of people who are convinced they hear
differences in cables (in sighted conditions) (shouldn't be too hard to
find such a group).
2. Run that group through a DBT of those cables. Now take the subset
of the group that can't distinguish the cables. (Given enough trials,
this should be about 100% of the group, right?)
3. Return that group to their previous condition --- sighted
listening.

Now, predict:
What percentage of the group will still believe they hear a difference
between the cables in sighted listening?

100% ?? Nobody's expectation bias will be affected by their DBT
experience?

0% ?? Nobody will have expectation bias anymore, or their expectation
bias will now be "there is no difference" ??

Somewhere in-between? In that case, what additional information would
you need to predict their response?

If the response is 100% still believing they hear a difference, what
additional information or experience whould they need to change their
perception?

Does their response depend on their opinion of the DBT's validity?

Has anybody actually done this experiment? Have anybody done similar
experiments?
  #33   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

wrote:
First, I'd like to thank those who have responded (except for the guy
that called me a troll).

But so far, nobody has attempted to answer my original question.
I'm trying to understand expectation bias. I'm trying to find out

how
well it is understood. I'm curious how much progress has been made

in
really understanding it, and if it can (has?) been characterized
sufficiently for engineering purposes.

To me, that means that we have sufficient analytical skills and
knowledge to predict the outcome of experiments that involve
expectation bias. For example, I was wondering how robust

expectation
bias is, and in particular how effective DBT experience might be in
changing expectation bias.

So I ask for predictions of the outcome of the following experiment.

1. Start with a group of people who are convinced they hear
differences in cables (in sighted conditions) (shouldn't be too hard

to
find such a group).
2. Run that group through a DBT of those cables. Now take the

subset
of the group that can't distinguish the cables. (Given enough

trials,
this should be about 100% of the group, right?)
3. Return that group to their previous condition --- sighted
listening.

Now, predict:
What percentage of the group will still believe they hear a

difference
between the cables in sighted listening?

100% ?? Nobody's expectation bias will be affected by their DBT
experience?

0% ?? Nobody will have expectation bias anymore, or their

expectation
bias will now be "there is no difference" ??

Somewhere in-between? In that case, what additional information

would
you need to predict their response?

If the response is 100% still believing they hear a difference, what
additional information or experience whould they need to change their
perception?

Does their response depend on their opinion of the DBT's validity?

Has anybody actually done this experiment? Have anybody done similar
experiments?


All I can offer is an anecdote. (But remember: the plural of anecdote
is data.) I was introduced to the green pen in an audio shop some years
ago. It sounded like a total crock to me, but I let the guy play me an
untreated CD (Barbra Streisand--ugh!), then color the rim and play it
again. Damn if it didn't sound better the second time! I don't think
doing a DBT could have made me any more skeptical than I already was,
and yet I heard a difference.

Based on that, I'd say that doing DBTs might change people's attitudes
to the extent that it would make them skeptical of what they heard in
sighted comparisons, but it wouldn't change what they heard in those
sighted comparisons. (And I suspect a fair number of them would
rationalize away the results of the DBT and continue to believe what
they heard sighted.)

bob
  #34   Report Post  
Nousaine
 
Posts: n/a
Default

wrote:

First, I'd like to thank those who have responded (except for the guy
that called me a troll).

But so far, nobody has attempted to answer my original question.
I'm trying to understand expectation bias. I'm trying to find out how
well it is understood. I'm curious how much progress has been made in
really understanding it, and if it can (has?) been characterized
sufficiently for engineering purposes.


I think you just haven't been willing to accept the useful information that has
been offered.

Of course there is understanding as to human bias mechanisms, of which
expectation of difference is but one, but they cannot have engineering value if
they have no physical effect. Their usefulness lay in the marketing, style and
sale technique domain.

I refer you to "Inevitable Illusions" (Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini) subtiltled
"How Mistakes of Reason Rule Our Minds" which describes human perceptual bias
and how it leads to mistaken interpretation of experience.

To me, that means that we have sufficient analytical skills and
knowledge to predict the outcome of experiments that involve
expectation bias. For example, I was wondering how robust expectation
bias is, and in particular how effective DBT experience might be in
changing expectation bias.


Your term 'expectation bias' is not well defined and is part of a larger set of
human physical and psychological characteristics which tend lead to mistakes in
intepretation of experience.

But let me propose a definition a human characteristic of human perception that
might to fairly called a form of expectation bias.

The very process of a comparative analysis has already been saddled with the
expectation of 'difference' otherwise why compare? The very idea of an audition
in a store has already planted an expectation of difference. And, as humans,
our perceptual bias leans toward an expectation of difference on first
stimulus. You know the old story ...when you hear a twig break behind you, you
immediately assume its a tiger and check back later. And if you run so far that
never look back you tend to forever assume that it actually was a tiger.


So I ask for predictions of the outcome of the following experiment.

1. Start with a group of people who are convinced they hear
differences in cables (in sighted conditions) (shouldn't be too hard to
find such a group).


Your problem will be cornering those people into actually undertaking the
experiment.

2. Run that group through a DBT of those cables. Now take the subset
of the group that can't distinguish the cables. (Given enough trials,
this should be about 100% of the group, right?)


3. Return that group to their previous condition --- sighted
listening.


If you reveal the results of the first test then you'll have a hard time
getting anybody to do it again. Your experiment will have to be much more
sophisticated to actually assess the effect.

Now, predict:
What percentage of the group will still believe they hear a difference
between the cables in sighted listening?
100% ??


That depends on a couple of other related factors. The time interval and the
prior and on-going investment in the idea. For someone who already has sizeable
sunk cost investment either psychological or on-going financial investment in
the concept there will be a tendency to discredit or discount the experimental
results or the impact of the newly found evidence will tend to wane over time
as the continuing influence of other biasing mechanisms (colleagues, press)
overcomes doubt.

Nobody's expectation bias will be affected by their DBT
experience?


Actually the 'expectation' bias you describe is seldom affected in any
meaningful way. The very act of comparing implies difference and the
discounters will tend to find fault with the expriment.


0% ?? Nobody will have expectation bias anymore, or their expectation
bias will now be "there is no difference" ??

Somewhere in-between? In that case, what additional information would
you need to predict their response?

If the response is 100% still believing they hear a difference, what
additional information or experience whould they need to change their
perception?


For people in this category its not a matter of changing perception (acoustical
perception has nothing to do with the belief as the results of the 1st
experiment clearly shows) its a matter of changing their interpretation of the
data.

Some human bias mechanisms are not changeable. For example the optical illusion
that the St Louis Arch is taller that it is wide is not changed when one
measures the dimensions. Its a replicable illusion, one where the perception is
clearly in error and that error can be known in advance.

So doesn't that mean that you can engineer
this bias into objects? Well it tells us that to get the perception that a
physical object is taller than wide doesn't mean that need to physically make
it that way. So? That's not reallyan engineering concept ... its a style and
image concept.

Does their response depend on their opinion of the DBT's validity?


Here's the advance discount of data and results idea at work. :-)


Has anybody actually done this experiment? Have anybody done similar
experiments?


I've done similar experiments. Studying the effect of presentation order and
loudness in comparative sound judgements (31 subjects, 431 total trials) I
developed a set of 10 A/B musical segments of which 5 were exactly the same
loudness and 5 had 1 dB difference between A and B.

What I found was that subejcts were perfectly willing to find "differences"
(prefer A or B and not No Preference) 76% of the time when A and B were exactly
the same!! Surprisingly this tendency did not change when the subjects were
men-off-the-street or audio professionals.

I was also surprised that there was not a significant difference due to order
when the presentations were identical loudness. But the rate at which subjects
Prefered B was much stronger when B was 1-dB louder.

How does this relate to engineering? Not at all. How does this relate to sales
technique? Ever wonder why demonstrations in the Salon have the salesman
turning the volume all the way down between comparative demonstrations and them
back up by an apparently un-calibrated amount for the 2nd listening?

If you'll examine your own demonstration techniques when demonstrating your new
"Boys Toys" .... old first, new and better 2nd all without any consideration of
level matching. Its really easy to convince yourself of improvements, even if
the technique is running at a subconscious level, that may not actually have a
true sonic effect. After all where did you initially learn about audio
demonstrations if NOT at an audio store?

In my opinion, the expectation bias common to humans is a powerful force that
any enthusiast has been subjected to in an audio context and has had a large
influence on sales because its been easy to build into sales floor method, you
don't even have to know about it or understand it to know that the
demonstration technque (preferred product 2nd, all the way done between
presentations) has a positive effect on your sales performance.
  #35   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

I think the answer is yes, for two reasons:

1. There are entire departments or fields of Advertising, Marketing,
Industrial Design, Ergonomics, and Psychology at major universities.
These people have studied expectation bias extensively. "Managing
customer expectations" is part of the job of any skilled marketeer or
salesman. People are trained in how to do this.

2. The commercial success of products whose sole content is the
manipulation of expectation bias.

But you may be up against an issue of semantics -- among mainstream
engineers, manipulation of expectation bias is considered to be outside
of engineering. Having "expectation bias engineering" may be as much of
an oxymoron as "creation science."

wrote:

But so far, nobody has attempted to answer my original question.
I'm trying to understand expectation bias. I'm trying to find out

how
well it is understood. I'm curious how much progress has been made

in
really understanding it, and if it can (has?) been characterized
sufficiently for engineering purposes.



  #36   Report Post  
Ban
 
Posts: n/a
Default

wrote:

Based on that, I'd say that doing DBTs might change people's attitudes
to the extent that it would make them skeptical of what they heard in
sighted comparisons, but it wouldn't change what they heard in those
sighted comparisons. (And I suspect a fair number of them would
rationalize away the results of the DBT and continue to believe what
they heard sighted.)

bob


I am very careful about judgements done after listening tests. I know my
ears can fool me, as there are many biases, not only sighted expectation,
reasoning or other logic constructs, but also mood, fatigue or wellness.
Additionally we tend to overcompensate.

A real story:
when I was testing an additional subwoofer, I first made some measurements
on the listening position to find the right level, lets call it 0dB. I then
listened to my favourite tracks and had the feeling of missing bass, so I
turned up the subwoofer volume by +2.5dB. After a couple of hours, I
realized the bass is too strong and turned it down to +1.5dB. This happened
repeatedly and after one week I arrived at, guess what?

I found this mike to be utterly useful, because it gives a consistent
reading independent of any bias, and when you know what the proper setting
should be, you can rely much more on it than your own perception. An even
frequency response corresponds amazingly well with the optimal listening
sensation.
The success of Canadian speaker manufacturers also seems to indicate this.
They have an exellent low-reflection room at disposition with all the
measurement gear, provided by the local government agency. So I would
declare *properly done and interpreted* measurements superiour to listening
tests alone.

--
ciao Ban
Bordighera, Italy
  #37   Report Post  
Stewart Pinkerton
 
Posts: n/a
Default

On 10 Dec 2004 01:08:21 GMT, wrote:

All I can offer is an anecdote. (But remember: the plural of anecdote
is data.) I was introduced to the green pen in an audio shop some years
ago. It sounded like a total crock to me, but I let the guy play me an
untreated CD (Barbra Streisand--ugh!), then color the rim and play it
again. Damn if it didn't sound better the second time! I don't think
doing a DBT could have made me any more skeptical than I already was,
and yet I heard a difference.


That is exactly how expectation bias works, and if a thousand people
'hear' the same thing, it's not data, it's just confirmation that
expectation bias works - but we already knew that.

OTOH, I can offer a DBT. Four of us got together many years ago, after
hours in a record store, and we had six identical copies of Dave
Brubeck's 'Late Night Brubeck', an Arcam carousel player, and a rather
nice system comprising a Naim amplifier and a pair of Epos ES11
speakers, carefully set up in the store (one of those 'enthusiast'
record stores where the owner was an audiophile as well as a record
buff). After checking that all the naked CDs sounded the same (and
rejecting two that semed a little different), two of the discs were
'greened', and the four discs were loaded into the player by one of
the participants. Another, who hadn't seen the discs loaded, was
assigned to the remote control, to play the same track from the four
discs in random order, while we all ticked off score sheets for
'green' or 'untreated', and the operator/listener noted which disc was
playing on each occasion. Two exhausting hours later, we compared
notes over a few beers, and the result was that we had a completely
random scatter, with no apparent sonic effect whatever from the green
pen.
--

Stewart Pinkerton | Music is Art - Audio is Engineering
  #38   Report Post  
Harry Lavo
 
Posts: n/a
Default

"Stewart Pinkerton" wrote in message
...
On 10 Dec 2004 01:08:21 GMT, wrote:

All I can offer is an anecdote. (But remember: the plural of anecdote
is data.) I was introduced to the green pen in an audio shop some years
ago. It sounded like a total crock to me, but I let the guy play me an
untreated CD (Barbra Streisand--ugh!), then color the rim and play it
again. Damn if it didn't sound better the second time! I don't think
doing a DBT could have made me any more skeptical than I already was,
and yet I heard a difference.


That is exactly how expectation bias works, and if a thousand people
'hear' the same thing, it's not data, it's just confirmation that
expectation bias works - but we already knew that.

OTOH, I can offer a DBT. Four of us got together many years ago, after
hours in a record store, and we had six identical copies of Dave
Brubeck's 'Late Night Brubeck', an Arcam carousel player, and a rather
nice system comprising a Naim amplifier and a pair of Epos ES11
speakers, carefully set up in the store (one of those 'enthusiast'
record stores where the owner was an audiophile as well as a record
buff). After checking that all the naked CDs sounded the same (and
rejecting two that semed a little different), two of the discs were
'greened', and the four discs were loaded into the player by one of
the participants. Another, who hadn't seen the discs loaded, was
assigned to the remote control, to play the same track from the four
discs in random order, while we all ticked off score sheets for
'green' or 'untreated', and the operator/listener noted which disc was
playing on each occasion. Two exhausting hours later, we compared
notes over a few beers, and the result was that we had a completely
random scatter, with no apparent sonic effect whatever from the green
pen.



Just out of curiousity, did you listen "sighted" first and did you determine
what you thought you were hearing / what you were "listening for"?
  #39   Report Post  
Stewart Pinkerton
 
Posts: n/a
Default

On 11 Dec 2004 16:10:10 GMT, "Harry Lavo" wrote:

"Stewart Pinkerton" wrote in message
...
On 10 Dec 2004 01:08:21 GMT, wrote:

All I can offer is an anecdote. (But remember: the plural of anecdote
is data.) I was introduced to the green pen in an audio shop some years
ago. It sounded like a total crock to me, but I let the guy play me an
untreated CD (Barbra Streisand--ugh!), then color the rim and play it
again. Damn if it didn't sound better the second time! I don't think
doing a DBT could have made me any more skeptical than I already was,
and yet I heard a difference.


That is exactly how expectation bias works, and if a thousand people
'hear' the same thing, it's not data, it's just confirmation that
expectation bias works - but we already knew that.

OTOH, I can offer a DBT. Four of us got together many years ago, after
hours in a record store, and we had six identical copies of Dave
Brubeck's 'Late Night Brubeck', an Arcam carousel player, and a rather
nice system comprising a Naim amplifier and a pair of Epos ES11
speakers, carefully set up in the store (one of those 'enthusiast'
record stores where the owner was an audiophile as well as a record
buff). After checking that all the naked CDs sounded the same (and
rejecting two that semed a little different), two of the discs were
'greened', and the four discs were loaded into the player by one of
the participants. Another, who hadn't seen the discs loaded, was
assigned to the remote control, to play the same track from the four
discs in random order, while we all ticked off score sheets for
'green' or 'untreated', and the operator/listener noted which disc was
playing on each occasion. Two exhausting hours later, we compared
notes over a few beers, and the result was that we had a completely
random scatter, with no apparent sonic effect whatever from the green
pen.



Just out of curiousity, did you listen "sighted" first and did you determine
what you thought you were hearing / what you were "listening for"?


No, we only listened blind, and we were listening for *any*
differences. Two of the 'panel', including the store owner, were
convinced that green pens made the sound smoother.
--

Stewart Pinkerton | Music is Art - Audio is Engineering
  #40   Report Post  
 
Posts: n/a
Default

This is effectively the perfect argument against DBT -- the testing
environment is psychologically invasive in a way that disables our
perception of audio effects. How convenient. It is also a stock
argument against objective testing of paranormal effects in general.
What it does is to eliminate the possibility of further discussion.

People can train themselves to perform amazing feats in psychologically
hostile environments. For instance, you can learn to trick a polygraph
test (it helps that polygraph testing is a crock to begin with). There
are people who have reportedly figured out how to detect WMD's in
aerial photographs while everybody in the world watches on live TV.
This is analogous to hearing the effect of the green pen or the magic
stones. If that is possible, then surely someone willing to win a
million bucks can train themselves to detect the magic stones in a DBT.

wrote:

All I can offer is an anecdote. (But remember: the plural of anecdote
is data.) I was introduced to the green pen in an audio shop some

years
ago. It sounded like a total crock to me, but I let the guy play me

an
untreated CD (Barbra Streisand--ugh!), then color the rim and play it
again. Damn if it didn't sound better the second time! I don't think
doing a DBT could have made me any more skeptical than I already was,
and yet I heard a difference.

Based on that, I'd say that doing DBTs might change people's

attitudes
to the extent that it would make them skeptical of what they heard in
sighted comparisons, but it wouldn't change what they heard in those
sighted comparisons. (And I suspect a fair number of them would
rationalize away the results of the DBT and continue to believe what
they heard sighted.)

bob

Reply
Thread Tools
Display Modes

Posting Rules

Smilies are On
[IMG] code is On
HTML code is Off


Similar Threads
Thread Thread Starter Forum Replies Last Post
Step up transformer Wolverine High End Audio 2 March 9th 04 06:09 PM
rec.audio.car FAQ (Part 4/5) Ian D. Bjorhovde Car Audio 0 March 6th 04 06:54 AM
FS HEWLETT-PACKARD 204D Audio Oscillator with Step Attenuator Mike Nowlen Pro Audio 0 February 22nd 04 07:00 PM
step by step photo guides johnjay69 Car Audio 3 November 7th 03 11:11 PM
FS HEWLETT-PACKARD 204D Audio Oscillator with 10 dB Step Attenuator Mike Nowlen Pro Audio 0 November 1st 03 05:59 PM


All times are GMT +1. The time now is 04:12 PM.

Powered by: vBulletin
Copyright ©2000 - 2024, Jelsoft Enterprises Ltd.
Copyright ©2004-2024 AudioBanter.com.
The comments are property of their posters.
 

About Us

"It's about Audio and hi-fi"